SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.83 número197Differences between the use of ferric sulphate and ferric chloride on biodesulfurization of a large coal particleChanges in EEG amplitude (Alpha and Beta waves) with Thermal environment índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Journal

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Em processo de indexaçãoCitado por Google
  • Não possue artigos similaresSimilares em SciELO
  • Em processo de indexaçãoSimilares em Google

Compartilhar


DYNA

versão impressa ISSN 0012-7353

Dyna rev.fac.nac.minas vol.83 no.197 Medellín maio/jun. 2016

https://doi.org/10.15446/dyna.v83n197.57589 

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/dyna.v83n197.57589

Human error and response to alarms in process safety

Error humano y reacción a alarmas en seguridad de procesos

 

Beata Mrugalska a, Salman Nazir b, Edwin Tytyk c & Kjell Ivar Øvergård d

 

a Faculty of Engineering Management, Poznan University of Technology, Poznan, Poland, beata.mrugalska@put.poznan.pl
b Training and Assessment Research Group (TARG), Department of Maritime Technology and Innovation, Buskerud and Vestfold University College, Borre, Norway, salman.nazir@hbv.no
c Faculty of Engineering Management, Poznan University of Technology, Poznan, Poland, edwin.tytyk@put.poznan.pl
d Training and Assessment Research Group (TARG), Department of Maritime Technology and Innovation, Buskerud and Vestfold University College, Borre, Norway, Kjell.Oevergaard@hbv.no

 

Received: November 30th, 2015. Received in revised form: February 25th, 2016. Accepted: April 25th, 2016.

 

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.


Abstract
Human operators are required to respond to alarms in normal conditions, and also to find solutions to unexpected situations in real time. The aim of this study is to increase the knowledge of human responses to alarms in the context of occupational situations. It describes how humans contribute to accidents, and pays special attention to the assurance of process safety assurance, which is in part realized by timely reactions to system alarms. An experiment involving an operator´s reaction times to alarm signals was undertaken to investigate whether there are differential responses to visual as opposed to auditory alarms. The findings in the research show that visual alarm indicators are perceived faster than auditory signals. Furthermore, there was a significant negative correlation between the number of errors and reaction time, indicating an individual difference in error-proneness when reacting to visual alarms in a supervisory task.

Keywords: alarms, auditory signal; visual signal; human error; process safety; reaction time.

Resumen
Operadores humanos responden a alarmas en condiciones normales y también en situaciones inesperadas y anormales en tiempo real. El objetivo de este estudio consiste en aumentar el conocimiento de las reacciones humanas a alarmas en el ámbito laboral. Describe como los humanos pueden contribuir a accidentes y presta atención a la seguridad del proceso que se efectúa en el tiempo de reacción a las alarmas de sistema. Este experimento fue ejecutado para investigar si hay diferencias en respuestas diferenciales a alarmas visuales comparándolas a alarmas auditivas. Los resultados indican que las alarmas visuales son percibidas de manera más eficiente que las alarmas auditivas. Incluso, se pudo ver una correlación negativa entre la cantidad de errores y el tiempo de reacción, lo que indica una diferencia individual en su disposición a errores cuando reaccionan a alarmas visuales en operaciones de supervisión.

Palabras Clave: alarmas; señales auditivas; señales visuales; error humano; seguridad de procesos; tiempo de reacción.


 

1. Introduction

Technological progress has contributed to more advanced human-machine systems. However, in complex systems, human error is still perceived as a cause or contributing factor to in approximately 90% of accidents. Most of these accidents could have been prevented by safety management measures [1]. This is particularly visible in the case of accidents in construction and manufacturing, in which errors are viewed as inappropriate human decisions that have a negative impact on the operation of technical systems. Thereby they impact system performance by reducing safety and effectiveness [2]. Management of human errors is commonly focused on training, motivation, hardware design, and management systems, all of which can lead to improved safety performance [3,4].

In this paper, the role of humans in system failures in an industrial setting is discussed, and the particular element of alarm perception and reaction is tested in an experiment. The study aims to illustrate the effect of different alarm signals on human operators' reaction times and identify alarms for which the response was defined according to a pre-defined code. Reaction time and the correctness of responses for compound reactions were assessed for auditory and visual signals in a laboratory simulation situation. The final part of the paper formulates recommendations for overall system performance in machinery and equipment design.

 

2. Human contribution to occupational accidents

Theories on the causes of occupational accidents have changed over the years. However, two basic perspectives on human error and human contribution to accidents can be identified [5,6]. The first approach, hereafter named the "old" approach began at the start of the 20th century. This approach states that "…certain individuals are always more likely than others to sustain accidents, even though exposed to equal risk…" [7, p.1]. The basic assumption in the 'old' approach was to see engineered systems as fundamentally safe, while unreliable humans were the primary threats to safety. According to this approach, the employer was not responsible for bad working conditions, and employees were accused of causing accidents [8]. The way to improve safety was to protect the systems through the training of operators, by keeping discipline, and through the application of procedures and through the selection of appropriate workers (e.g. by employing the less accident prone workers). In more recent times, this approach involved introducing automation in industrial processes [5]. It was not until the second half of the twentieth century that a system was seen as a factor that contributed to accidents. Employers were also required to provide safe working conditions for the first time. At the same time, it was realised that a person's accident proneness may differ from one period to another [7,9]. Moreover, research has shown that dismissing workers due to their accident proneness does not positively affect the ratio of occupational accidents [10]. The problem of accident proneness remains a widely discussed topic [11] and studies show that operators' accident proneness only refers to a small amount of accidents, for which the application of preventive safety barriers can reduce the involved risks [12].

Alternatively, according to the new approach, human error is no longer seen as a cause, but rather as a symptom of an underlying systemic weakness or failure. Hence, human error is not a cause in itself but it rather is something that must be explained. In contrast to the assumptions of the old approach, the new approach holds that safety is not intrinsic in systems as systems are created to fulfil multiple incompatible human goals simultaneously. Many of these goals can be conflicting, as is exemplified by the wish to improve safety and efficiency simultaneously (e.g. reducing relative number of errors while also increasing production). Human error is clearly associated with features of tools, tasks and operating environments. Therefore, human beings and their interaction with technology are important for creating safety and progress in terms of safety levels [5]. Thus, there is need for further investigation of human error and organizational deficiencies, design problems, and procedural shortcomings. Following on from this, it can be claimed that the new approach to human error shows a significant movement towards human factors and points to new ways of managing organizational safety.

As there are different approaches to the causes of occupational accidents, the literature review allowed us to make a distinction between different accident theories, as are summarised Table 1.

These models provide an insight into how and why accidents happen in organization. However, it should be stressed that there are only a few accident models that are based on human factors research [11]. They refer to different factors that appear in many forms; however, generally they reflect human and technical aspects, when a human being can be recognized as assurance of safety. It should also be mentioned that the technical system design can be controlled/designed to reduce the occurrence of human errors, whereas the control of environmental factors and manner of human operation of the system are somewhat less controllable. Predominantly, in large organizations and compound technologies, more than one sin gle liability can be distinguished [5,8,15,18].

 

3. Process safety performance

Over the last three decades, automation has changed the role of operators from manual control to supervision [10]. A great effort has been put into equipment design and interface, operation, operator selection and training and behavioral enforcement. These efforts have resulted in a drop of process safety accidents. There is still a need to operate complex systems with the recognition of operation breakdowns that occur as a result of human error [18]. It should be emphasized that the main category of human error is operating errors. For example, the following situations can result in operating errors [19]:

  • lack of appropriate procedures
  • task complexity and overload conditions
  • poor personnel selection and training
  • operator carelessness and lack of interest
  • poor environmental conditions
  • departure from the correct operating procedures

In order to monitor the performance of systems the operators have to do many tasks that can rely on [20]:

  • monitoring sensor data from components (voltages, status of the capacitors, transmission lines, transformers, circuit breakers, etc.)
  • monitoring the grid as a holistic system
  • communicating information among several sub-systems
  • responding to alarms

Alarm systems play a significant role in industrial production as they assure process quality and reliability. Industrial production requires human operators who observe, and, if necessary, intervene in process malfunction or abnormal system performance i.e. when the values of the system parameters exceed predefined tolerance limits, or approach critical levels [21,22]. However, nowadays a large number of notifications, which are configured inside such a system, lead to the operator receiving much more alerts than s/he can physically and cognitively perceive. Due to the material, energy and information flow in a plant, particular disturbances can cause multiple subsequent alarm messages, which may overload the operator with redundant alarms. This results in an "alarm flood", when an operator is not able to react within a recommended response time and find the root cause of disturbances. Finally, the industrial applications of alarm systems become unreliable due to their complexity [23].

In order to increase knowledge of human reactions to unreliable signals, researchers refer to theories of learning and human cognition such as:

  • Probability matching - the approximation of the perceived true alarm rate on the basis of the observed alarm responses
  • Signal Detection Theory - signal detection and response is more rapid for historically reliable alarm systems
  • Automation trust - the exhibited trust relates to behavioral patterns of system use and misuse

Regardless of what the operation task is, alarm reaction patters are unquestionably influenced by perceived alarm system reliability [24].

According to Lee and See [25] reliability of the alarm system is described as the percentage of critical events that are correctly identified by the alarm system. It is determined by the alarm's sensitivity, setting of thresholds and defined probability of dangerous conditions [26]. The higher the alarm system's reliability is, the more that operators can rely on the alarm and the less monitoring the data is required. On the other hand, when reliability is low, the chance of the occurrence of false alarms or misses is higher and the relevant process data have to be monitored more frequently to be able to keep monitoring performance at a high level [27,28]. Unreliable systems can contribute to sensor failure or their inadequate performances, data processing limitations or output failure (i.e. electrical problems or component malfunction, etc.). All these states can lead to potentially hazardous situations as they cause more deviations in other parts of the plant [23].

 

4. Human reaction to alarms

Displays and controls have been used for a long time in human-machine interfaces. They range from simple to complex manufacturing systems and processes. There are also distinct sets of human factor principles for choosing frequency, duration, and intensity of signal presentation. In order to obtain attention and a response, the two most common modalities can be differentiated, which are visual and auditory signals.

In everyday life, most information is perceived with the eyes; however, in complex systems, where the object of the task is out of visual view, the use of auditory stimuli has become more common. Therefore, in industrial control visual and auditory signals are often presented together rather than as alternatives; however, they may also be separate or simultaneous, synchronous or asynchronous [29,30]. They can be used in hand and/or foot controlled communication devices. The foot controls are mainly designed for seated operators when there is a need to reduce the workload on the hands, or when free hands are required for tasks with greater levels of precision and skill. However, it should be emphasized that even in the case of equipment or systems that have foot controls (i.e. automobiles, airplanes), nearly all controls are assigned for hand control [31].

The literature studies revealed that the timing of auditory stimuli is generally longer than the timing of visual stimuli for the same signals presented separately. Furthermore, duration discrimination is more precise for auditory than for visual stimuli in simultaneous presentations [32]. It was also identified that the time perception relies on the prefrontal and parietal regions, and on the auditory and visual cortices. However, the neural mechanism responsible for these modalities remains unclear [33]. Moreover, in recognition of visual and auditory signals, the phenomenon called visual dominance [34] must be taken into account. Based on this, auditory signals may seem not to be responded to in simultaneous presentation with visual signals, which indicates that their use can be ineffective in control situations. Nevertheless, in many situations, it was recognized that they might stimulate situational awareness and thereby improve the effectiveness of visual displays. They are superior to visual signals for warning in monitoring tasks in which there is no need to observe a particular location all the time [35].

The level of urgency of alarms is correlated with the level of urgency of the indicated danger [36]. Furthermore, the perception of alarms is subject to their frequency, intensity, and repetition. For instance, a low frequency alarm, which is repeated slowly, shows low urgency, whereas a high frequency alarm repeated quickly implies high urgency [37]. In simulated conditions it was shown that the reduction of luminous intensity from 42 to 5 cd increases the reaction time by 84 ms [38]. In the case of auditory signals, increased signal intensity causes faster reaction time, whereas its high-intensity, which is synchronous with a visual signal, reduces choice reaction time [39]. Thus, the growth of the intensity of auditory signal weakens the visual supremacy. For synchronous auditory and visual tasks, sound source location does not have a facilitating effect. It was concluded that sound not only warns, but also has an overall alerting effect. Auditory signals are quite useful when the spatial-visual information is limited or chaotic. For instance, they can be applied in control console tasks in which they alert operators or request action. While listening to critical auditory messages, visual signals are monitored. To optimize their reaction rate and reduce their performance lag, a fore-period warning (at least 2s and no more than 4s) is required to draw attention to incoming signals. Both signals should be simultaneously presented from the same direction to ensure quick responses. Nevertheless, in the case of asynchronous signals, a short delay (200 ms) between them may give a negative result, giving a poor response speed and accuracy [29].

Base on the literature study, it was hypothesized that reaction times for visual alarms would be significantly shorter than reaction times for audible alarms. It was also postulated that audible alarms would have a higher percentage of errors than visual alarms.

 

5. Materials and methods

Eighty-seven second-year students from engineering management and safety engineering courses in Poznan University of Technology participated in the experiment. The sample consisted of 36 males and 51 females, with a mean age of 20.16 years (SD = 0.37). They did not report having hearing loss or color vision deficiency. In order to have free movements they were asked to wear casual and comfortable clothing. As the experiment was conducted during their laboratory classes, all the students were awarded a course credit.

At the beginning of the experiment the task was clearly explained, and then participants were allowed to practice until they became familiarized with it. Their individual training lasted for about 5 minutes. Afterwards, they experienced 3 visual and 1 auditory signals while operating the simulator (MCR-2001E). The experimental task was to react to 50 signals according to the previously defined code, which is shown in Table 2. When the experimental task started, a single auditory or visual target signal appeared on its own for each test trial. No feedback on the accuracy of responses was given to the participants.

Only reaction times for correct responses were recorded. Incorrect responses were given a reaction time of zero (0) and treated as a missing variable in the statistical analysis. Mean reaction time was calculated by summing up all the correct response times for each participant and then dividing this sum by the number of correct responses for visual and auditory alarms respectively.

IBM SPSS 22 was used for statistical analysis. All confidence intervals for r (Pearson correlation) were calculated using the bootstrap function in SPSS, and there were 1000 resamplings that had replacement.

 

6. Results and discussion

Visual alarms (M = 524.67 ms, 99% CI [506.39, 540.73]) had a significantly shorter reaction time than audible alarms (M = 579.27 ms, 99% CI [552.88, 608.64]) as measured by a paired samples t-test (t(86) = -6.336, Mdiff = -54.61 ms, 99% CI of Mdiff [-77.31, -31.90], p < .001). This is in accordance with the literature review and the stated expectations of this study.

Following this difference in mean reaction time, a difference in delayed (but correct) responses was expected. Delayed responses where defined as responses with more than 500 ms reaction time. Visual alarms had a much lower ratio of delayed responses (M = 42.78%, 99% CI [37.37%, 49.47%]) than audible alarms (M = 57.12%, 99% CI [49.01%, 65.57%]). This difference was statistically significant as measured by a paired samples t-test (t(86) = -4.869, Mdiff = -14.33%, 99% CI of Mdiff [-22.09%, -6.58%], p < .001).

Regarding the number of errors, a paired samples t-test showed that there was no difference in the ratio of errors for visual alarms (M = 11.30%, 99% CI [9.35%, 13.49%]) as compared to audible alarms (M = 10.49%, 99% CI [7.61%, 13.36%]). The difference was not significant (t(86) = 0.616, Mdiff = -0.008%, 99% CI of Mdiff [-0.027, 0.043], p = .54). This is not in accordance with the hypothesized difference that audible alarms would be more prone to errors.

When the relationship between the rate of errors and reaction time was further investigated, a statistically significant negative correlation was found between reaction time for visual alarms and the ratio of errors to correct responses (r(87)= -.301, 95% CI [-.484, -.085]). This indicated that the participants who on average responded faster to visual alarms also tended to make a larger number of errors; therefore, this indicated an individual difference in error-proneness when reacting to visual alarms in a supervisory task.

For audible alarms, the same negative correlation between reaction times and ratio of errors was observed, but this relationship was not statistically significant (r(87)=-0.95, 95% CI [-.223, .117]). The lack of a significant negative correlation in this study could be due to the relatively lower number of auditory alarms (8 in total) compared to visual alarms (42 in total).

This study employed a non-randomized convenience sample, hence limiting the ability to make causal inferences outside the current sample. However, our findings are still in accordance with the existing literature on this matter.

The limited number of audible alarms has restricted our ability to identify real differences in error rates when responding to audible alarms. This limitation can explain our lack of ability to statistically identify differences between error rates as well as the lack of a significant negative correlation between reaction time and error rates for audible alarms. This limitation can be amended in future studies by balancing the number of visual to audible alarms.

 

7. Conclusions

In any process industry, safety issues are of primary importance as any accident can have cataclysmic consequences for both people and the environment. Therefore, efforts must be made to prevent or limit the hazards of the plant. To achieve this aim, an increasing number of industrial quality and reliability systems is applied. In these systems, operators pay attention to present process states and predict their future states using information from alarm systems. Alarms are frequently performed via display terminals in which information is presented in a variety of formats. In this paper, particular attention has been paid to visual and audible signals. The results of the present study revealed that the participants could react faster to visual signals than audible ones when the signals are predefined according to a code. No difference was found in the reaction of errors compared with the total number of alarms.

These results aim to be helpful in formulating recommendations to work towards a more optimal design of control consoles when auditory and visual displays are presented. They may be also effective in improving system performance in machinery and equipment design.

 

References

[1] Leva, M.C., Human errors analysis and safety management systems in hazardous activities. [Online]. (Interim Report IR-05e003), 2005. Available at: http://webarchive.iiasa.ac.at/Admin/PUB/Documents/IR-05-003.pdf.         [ Links ]

[2] Sanders, M. and McCormick, E., Human factors in engineering and design. New York: McGraw-Hill 1993.         [ Links ]

[3] Górny, A., Ergonomics in the formation of work condition quality. Work. A Journal of Prevention, Assessment and Rehabilitation, 41, pp. 1708-1711, 2013.         [ Links ]

[4] Belu, N., Misztal, A., Butlewski, M. and Rachieru, N., The directions for improvement in Polish and Romanian enterprises in applying the principles of Social Responsibility, Proceedings of the 4th Review of Management and Economic Engineering International Management Conference, pp. 339-344, 2014.         [ Links ]

[5] Dekker, S.W.A., Reconstructing human contributions to accidents: The new view on error and performance. Journal of Safety Research, 33, pp. 371-385, 2002. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-4375(02)00032-4        [ Links ]

[6] Sanmiquel-Pera, L., Vintro, C. and Freijo, M., Characteristics of the 3 most common types of occupational accident in Spanish sub-surface and surface mining, from 2003-2008. DYNA, 79(172), pp. 118-125, 2012.         [ Links ]

[7] Froggatt, P. and Smiley, J.A., The concept of accident proneness: A review. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 21(1), pp. 1-12, 1964. DOI: 10.         [ Links ]1136/oem.21.1.1

[8] Mrugalska, B., Nazir, S., Tytyk, E. and Øvergård, K.I., Process safety control for occupational accident prevention, in: Arezes, P.M., Baptista, J.S., Barroso, M.P. et al. (eds.) Occupational Safety and Hygiene III, (SHO), London: Taylor and Francis Group, 2015. pp. 365-369. DOI: 10.1201/b18042-74        [ Links ]

[9] Heinrich, H.W., Industrial accident. London: McGraw-Hill, 1959.         [ Links ]

[10] Hämäläinen, P., Global estimates of occupational accidents and fatal work-related diseases. PhD Thesis, Tampere University of Technology, Tampere, Finland, 2010.         [ Links ]

[11] Nazir, S. and Manca, D., How a plant simulator can improve industrial safety. Process Safety Progress, 34(3), pp. 237-243, 2015, DOI: 10.1002/prs.11714. DOI: 10.1002/prs.11714        [ Links ]

[12] Kjellen, U., Prevention of accidents through experience feedback. London: Taylor & Francis Group, 2000. DOI: 10.1201/b17206        [ Links ]

[13] Heinrich, H.W., Industrial accident prevention. London: McGraw-Hill, 1931.         [ Links ]

[14] Bird, F.E.J., and Germain, G.L., Practical loss control leadership. Loganville, Geogia: International Loss Control Institute, Inc., 1985.         [ Links ]

[15] Reason, J., Human error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9781139062367        [ Links ]

[16] Rasmussen, J., Risk management in a dynamic society. Safety Science, 27, pp. 183-213, 1997. DOI: 10.1016/S0925-7535(97)00052-0        [ Links ]

[17] Leveson, N., A new accident model for engineering safer systems. Safety Science, 42, pp. 237-270, 2004. DOI: 10.1016/S0925-7535(03)00047-X        [ Links ]

[18] Grigoras, G. and Barbulescu, C., Human errors monitoring in electrical transmission networks based on a partitioning algorithm. Electrical Power and Energy Systems, 49, pp. 128-136, 2013. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijepes.2012.12.016        [ Links ]

[19] Mc Leod, J.E.N. and Rivera, S., Human error management optimization in CAREM NPP. Proceedings of the World Congress on Engineering (WCE), pp. 618-622, 2009.         [ Links ]

[20] Su, R. and Yurcik, W., A survey and comparison of human monitoring of complex network. National Center for Computing Applications (NCSA), [Online]. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Available at: http://www.dodccrp.org/events/10th_ICCRTS/CD/papers/342.pdf.         [ Links ]

[21] Manca, D., Nazir, S., Colombo, S. and Kluge, A., Procedure for automated assessment of industrial operators. Chemical Engineering Transactions, 36, pp. 391-396, 2014.         [ Links ]

[22] Mrugalska, B., Akielaszek-Witczak, A. and Stetter, R., Robust quality control of products with experimental design. In D. Popescu (ed.), International Conference on Production Research - Regional Conference Africa, Europe and the Middle East and 3rd International Conference on Quality and Innovation in Engineering and Management, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, pp. 343-348, 2014.         [ Links ]

[23] Schleburg, M., Christiansen, L., Thornhill, N.F. and Fay, A., A combined analysis of plant connectivity and alarm logs to reduce the number of alerts in an automation system. Journal of Process Control, 23, pp. 839-851, 2013. DOI: 10.1016/j.jprocont.2013.03.010        [ Links ]

[24] Bliss, J.P. and Chancey, E.T., An investigation of training strategies to improve alarm reactions. Applied Ergonomics, 45, pp. 1278-1284, 2014. DOI: 10.1016/j.apergo.2013.06.007        [ Links ]

[25] Lee, J.D. and See, K.A., Trust in automation: designing for appropriate reliance. Hum. Factors, 46, pp. 50-80, 2004. DOI: 10.1518/hfes.46.1.50_30392        [ Links ]

[26] Bustamante, E.A., Bliss, J.P. and Anderson B.L., Effects of varying the threshold of alarm systems and workload on human performance. Ergonomics, 50(7), pp. 1127-1147, 2007. DOI: 10.1080/00140130701237345        [ Links ]

[27] Velez, J.C., Lishak, M.Y. and Antonov-Antipov, Y.N., False alarm and detection probabilities calculation of a digital unified detector. DYNA, 77(162), pp.271-280, 2010.         [ Links ]

[28] Onnasch, L., Ruff, S. and Manzey, D., Operators' adaptation to imperfect automation - Impact of miss-prone alarm systems on attention allocation and performance. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 72(10-11), pp. 772-782, 2014. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2014.05.001        [ Links ]

[29] Lee, F.C.H. and Chan, A.H.S., Attending visual and auditory signals: Ergonomics recommendations with consideration of signal modality and spatial stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 37, pp. 197-206, 2007. DOI: 10.1016/j.ergon.2006.10.017        [ Links ]

[30] Tsang, S.N.H., Kang S.X.Q. and Chan, A.H.S., Spatial S-R compatibility effect with head rotation. Proceedings of the International MultiConference of Engineers and Computer Scientists (IMECS), pp. 1115-1119, 2014.         [ Links ]

[31] Chan, A.H.S. and Chan, K.W.L., Three-dimensional spatial stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility for visual signals with hand and foot controls. Applied Ergonomics, 41, pp. 840-848, 2010. DOI: 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.03.005        [ Links ]

[32] Wearden, J.H., Todd, N.P.M. and Jones, I.A., When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? Q. J. Exp. Psychol, 59, pp. 1709-1724, 2006. DOI: 10.1080/17470210500314729        [ Links ]

[33] Chen, Y., Huang, X., Luo, Y., Pengc, C. and Liu, C., Differences in the neural basis of automatic auditory and visual time perception: ERP evidence from an across-modal delayed response oddball task. Brain Research, 1325, pp. 100-111, 2010. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.040        [ Links ]

[34] Posner, M.I., Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, pp. 3-25, 1980. DOI: 10.1080/00335558008248231        [ Links ]

[35] Lee, F.C.H. and Chan, A.H.S., Ergonomics recommendations for simultaneous and delayed presentation of visual and auditory signals. Displays, 29, pp. 124-131, 2008. DOI: 10.1016/j.displa.2007.09.006        [ Links ]

[36] Arrabito, G.R., Mondor, T.A. and Kent K.J., Judging the urgency of non-verbal auditory alarms: a case study. Ergonomics, 47(8), pp. 821-840, 2004. DOI: 10.1080/0014013042000193282        [ Links ]

[37] Parasuraman, R., Hancock, P.A. and Olofinboba, O., Alarm effectiveness in driver-centred collision-warning systems. Ergonomics, 40, pp. 390-399, 1997. DOI: 10.1080/001401397188224        [ Links ]

[38] Sivak, M., Flannagan M.J., Sato, T., Traube E.C. and Aoki, M., Reaction times to neon, LED, and fast incandescent brake lamps. Ergonomics, 37(6), 1994. DOI: 10.1080/00140139408963712        [ Links ]

[39] Keuss, P.J.G., Van Der Zee, F. and Van Der Bree, M.B.M., Auditory accessory effects on visual processing. Acta Psychologica, 75(1), pp. 41-54, 1990. DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90065-N        [ Links ]

 

B. Mrugalska, received her MSc. Eng. in Corporate Management in 2001, her PhD. in Machines Building and Operations in 2008, from Poznan University of Technology, Poland. Since 2009, she has been working in the Chair of Ergonomics and Quality Engineering at the Faculty of Engineering Management in Poznan University of Technology. She is a Centre for Registration of European Ergonomists (CREE) Council representative in her capacity as a member of the Polish National Assessment Board. Her research interests include: robust design and control, parameter optimization techniques, system faults and failures, quality control, machinery safety, process safety and reliability. ORCID: 0000-0001-9827-9449

S. Nazir, is a researcher at Human Factors Research Group at Buskerud and Vestfold University College, where he works in the Faculty of Technology and Maritime Sciences. He received his MSc. and PhD in Chemical Engineering (focuses on System Engineering and Human Factors respectively) from Hanyang University, South Korea and Politecnico di Milano, Italy, respectively. His research interests include: human factors, training methods, virtual reality training simulators, performance indices, complex socio-technical systems, system safety, and distributed situation awareness. Dr. Nazir has won several awards, scholarships and some research grants during his career. He was awarded with Cum Laude (outstanding honor) when awarded his PhD. ORCID: 0000-0002-2058-6147

E. Tytyk, received his MSc. Eng. in Plastic Working in 1972, his PhD. degree in Machines Building in 1979, and then his Dr. in Machines Building and Ergonomics in 1991. He has worked as a full professor in Design Methodology and Ergonomics since 2004 in the Poznan University of Technology, Poland. Currently, he is a head of the Chair of Ergonomics and Quality Engineering at the Faculty of Engineering Management in Poznan University of Technology. He works as Full Professor both in the Poznan University of Technology and in the Komenski`s High School Academy in Leszno, Poland. His research interests include: ergonomic design methodology, ergonomic design for disabled and senior citizens, design of technical and organizational solutions to improve working conditions in different plants and technologies. ORCID: 0000-0001-9892-4380

K.I. Øvergård, received his BSc. in Welfare Nursing/Developmental Psychology from the University College of Akershus in 2001. He was awarded his MSc. in Psychology with a specialization in Ergonomics in 2004, and his PhD in Psychology - Cognitive Systems Engineering in 2008, from the Norwegian University of Science and Technology in Trondheim, Norway. Øvergård worked at the Accident Investigation Board of Norway and later at the Norwegian Railroad. In 2011 he was employed as Full Professor in Work and Organizational Psychology-Maritime Human Factors at Buskerud and Vestfold University College, Norway - where he is now the Head of Maritime Research at the Faculty of Technology and Maritime Sciences. In 2012 he became Chair of Cognitive Systems Engineering and Human Factors at the Global Maritime Knowledge Hub. His research interests include: maritime human factors, identifying characteristics and causes of organizational failures/resilience, applied statistics and involvement of human error in accidents. ORCID: 0000-0002-4029-4344

Creative Commons License Todo o conteúdo deste periódico, exceto onde está identificado, está licenciado sob uma Licença Creative Commons