SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.65 número3Uso de psicofármacos en indicaciones no aprobadas por la agencia regulatoria nacional de ColombiaPrevalencia y variables asociadas a la pediculosis capitis en un hogar infantil de Popayán, Colombia índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Revista de la Facultad de Medicina

versión impresa ISSN 0120-0011

rev.fac.med. vol.65 no.3 Bogotá jul./set. 2017

https://doi.org/10.15446/revfacmed.v65n3.57057 

Original papers

Clinical features of brain activation deficit in children

Características clínicas de niños con déficit de activación cerebral general

Berenice Luna-Villanueva1 

Yulia Solovieva1  * 

Emelia Lázaro-García1 

Luis Quintanar1 

1 Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla - Faculty of Psychology - Master's degree program in Neuropsychological Diagnosis and Rehabilitation - Puebla - México.


Abstract

Introduction:

Brain activation is considered as one of the mechanisms of the brain related to the functional state of deep subcortical structures. A deficit in this mechanism may be involved in behavioral disorders during development and school learning.

Objective:

To identify the clinical features of Mexican schoolchildren with general brain activation deficit, and to determine the neuropsychological tasks that help to detect this syndrome.

Materials and methods:

The sample included 20 Mexican schoolchildren attending regular schools and diagnosed with behavioral and/or learning disorders. The types of errors and performance in neuropsychological tasks were analyzed by categories.

Results:

The results showed that there are common clinical features in schoolchildren, particularly, executive instability, fatigue and/or slow execution, and instability in memory and perceptive graphic tasks. These features can be demonstrated through the qualitative syndromic analysis of perceptive graphic tasks, retention tasks and manual praxis.

Conclusions:

Qualitative assessment is effective to differentiate this type of cases from other possible neuropsychological conditions.

Keywords: Neuropsychology; Evaluation; Diagnosis (MeSH)

Resumen

Introducción.

La activación cerebral general se considera como un mecanismo de trabajo cerebral relacionado con el estado funcional de las estructuras subcorticales profundas; su déficit puede subyacer a los trastornos conductuales durante el desarrollo y el aprendizaje escolar.

Objetivos.

Precisar las características clínicas de escolares mexicanos con déficit de activación cerebral general e identificar las tareas neuropsicológicas que ayudan a la detección de este síndrome.

Materiales y métodos.

Se incluyeron 20 casos de escolares mexicanos: alumnos de escuelas regulares con problemas de conducta o aprendizaje que solicitaron apoyo de evaluación neuropsicológica. Se analizaron los tipos de error y la forma de ejecución de tareas neuropsicológicas por categorías.

Resultados.

Se demostró que existen características clínicas comunes en los escolares evaluados. Como rasgos particulares se identificó inestabilidad en la ejecución de tareas neuropsicológicas, fatiga o lentificación e inestabilidad en el mantenimiento de huellas mnésicas y durante la realización de tareas gráfico-perceptivas. Estos rasgos se evidenciaron a través del análisis sindrómico cualitativo en tareas gráfico-perceptivas, de retención en diversas modalidades y de praxias manuales.

Conclusión.

Se encontró eficacia clínica de diferenciación en este tipo de casos frente a otras posibles dificultades a nivel neuropsicológico.

Palabras clave: Neuropsicología; Evaluación; Diagnóstico (DeCS)

Introduction

According to Luria 1, the brain has three functional blocks, one of which is related to subcortical regulation levels. The author named this unit first functional block of the brain.

From a structural point of view of the first functional block, most of them are nuclei of the reticular formation located in the central part of the brainstem and in diverse parts of the thalamus 2. These nuclei have ascending and descending connections that are in charge of regulating the cortical tone, sleep and consciousness states, orientation reflex, stability, and the course of the mental processes. The literature shows that these structures are related to emotional, vegetative and mnesic regulation 3, and several opinions about their correlation with cognitive functioning can be found 4. The projections of the ascending reticular system reach non-specific nuclei in the thalamus, from where projections are sent to the cortex 2 to activate it and regulate the status of its activity.

As stated by Luria 1, the contribution of the first functional block is fundamental for maintaining the cortical tone necessary for performing any type of cognitive activity. This takes on a particular meaning in school age, when children invest most of their time in cognitive tasks that require executive stability, continuous attention and active stay during tasks. Thus, without a specific modal influence, the first functional block makes an uncertain but fundamental contribution to successful accomplishment of this type of tasks.

Accordingly, it is useful to consider deep subcortical structures as one of the fundamental neuropsychological factors involved in cognitive task performance at school age, which can be called general nonspecific activation factor 5. This factor is implicated in cortical energy activation and in general regulation of all mental processes 1; its functional weakness generates instability in the execution of cognitive actions. Such instability can be increased or expressed more clearly in fatigue states, loss of motivation, or increased complexity for executing an action 5.

Deep subcortical structures are particularly sensitive to adverse conditions in the perinatal and postnatal period. A deficient functional status of subcortical regulation levels is considered part of almost all immature syndromes or disorders in higher mental functions 6, preventing the nervous system from maintaining an adequate alert level and causing a general failure in information processing required to perform cognitive tasks 7.

At the neurophysiological level, several investigations have performed a qualitative analysis of electroencephalographic records in preschoolers 8-10 and schoolchildren 11-14 diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or learning problems 15-19. These works have identified a immaturity state in the nonspecific activation system 20,21, which is especially relevant since attentional processes fundamentally depend on the reticular formation 22. In the field of qualitative neuropsychology, several clinical studies have been carried out to identify behavioral and cognitive traits in children with these deficits; from this perspective, a neuropsychologist can identify the altered factor to understand the syndrome and provide tools for its reorganization 23-26.

Studies on preschool children diagnosed with ADHD have established that the functional weakness of the general nonspecific activation factor is one of the responsible factors. Typical errors and difficulties are related to fatigue, latency, short latencies to initiate an activity, difficulties in motor coordination activities, and executive instability in the presence of micrographs and macrographs 8,10. At school age, trace interruption, task abandonment, weak tracing and simplification have also been found 13,14. Difficulties in concentration and disturbances in attention have been described; in addition, greater fatigue caused by hyperactivity has also been observed as a compensation for the lack of stimulation of the central nervous system 6,27.

Furthermore, difficulties to evoke verbal and visual information of material semantically unrelated have also been observed in 7-and 8-year-old schoolchildren with immaturity in the nonspecific regulation system 20. The same observations have been made in the neuropsychological clinic of adults with brain lesions in deep subcortical structures 28-30. In this regard, Glozman 6 reports that the acquired information is unstable or inhibited by the interference of various influences, especially homogeneous influences, even with sufficient memory capacity.

The deficit of this neuropsychological factor can alter the performance in tasks and activities assigned during school learning, manifesting fatigue states before the complexity or duration of an activity increase 6. However, it is still necessary to systematize behavioral and cognitive clinical evidence that may help to define the presence of non-specific brain activation in cases of students with behavioral and learning problems. In particular, there are few data on these difficulties in school age, which makes the work of specialists difficult and does not allow differentiating these cases from those with a predominance of regulation and control problems. Meanwhile, none of the neuropsychological factors mentioned above can be identified in cases with an ADHD diagnosis, which makes differential neuropsychological diagnosis difficult, as well as decision making on treatment.

The objectives of this study are to characterize the clinical performance of schoolchildren with learning or behavioral problems who present deficits in the nonspecific brain activation factor, and to identify the neuropsychological tasks that help detecting this syndrome.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Twenty records of schoolchildren between 6 and 12 years of age were selected out of 105 patients attended due to learning or behavior problems in the neuropsychology unit of the university hospital of Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, México, in the period 2012-2015. Similar clinical features were identified in the selected cases, which allowed diagnosing functional deficit of nonspecific cerebral activation.

Out of 20 cases analyzed, 18 corresponded to males and six had been previously diagnosed with ADHD. The mean age was 8, and problems in school learning or behavior were the main cause for consultation. All children were public school students in Puebla.

None of the selected cases was related to pathological history (trauma or brain tumor) or to the presence of primary hearing impairment, vision or motor problems.

Material

All the participants were given the qualitative neuropsychological evaluation protocols "Puebla-Sevilla Child Neuropsychological Evaluation" 31 and "Verification of School Success in Primary School" 32. For data analysis, the tasks of both protocols were systematized by categories: graphic-perceptive, retention in different modality, manual coordination and intellectual tasks (Table 1).

Table 1 Neuropsychological and intellectual tasks analyzed. 

Source: Own elaboration based on Solovieva et al.31 and Solovieva & Quintanar 32.

Procedure

The types of error observed were recorded per subject, as well as the characteristics and difficulties found by the evaluator at the time of the assessment.

For the qualitative analysis, the responses were recorded and the most frequent types of errors and executions were characterized in the neuropsychological evaluation tasks. In addition, a comparison between tasks that showed typical errors and those that did not show any error was made. The tasks were classified in verbal, graphic-perceptive, retention, motor coordination and intellectual tasks. For the quantitative analysis, the types of errors and the most frequent execution characteristics were determined in the whole sample and in the different tasks of neuropsychological evaluation.

Results

A qualitative analysis of the clinical manifestations in neuropsychological tasks was performed. Table 2 shows the types of errors and the most common execution characteristics identified during the accomplishment of the tasks in each category.

Table 2 Summary of the types of errors and execution in neuropsycholo-gical tasks. 

Source: Own elaboration based on the data obtained in the study.

The different neuropsychological tasks provided interesting data. First, perceptual graphs showed types of errors such as micrographs, macrographs and loss ofhorizontality that were also evident in writing. Retention tasks showed difficulties to retain visual and auditory-visual information. Manual coordination tasks showed poor fluency in the movements; however, in some cases, the help of the evaluator was important to fulfill the task with better fluency. Finally, the execution of the students' verbal tasks improved when the evaluator repeated the information, even in the presence of phonemic substitutions.

Figure 1 presents some examples of the most common error types identified in perceptual graphic tasks.

Figure 1 Examples of common error types in perceptual graphic tasks. Source: Own elaboration based on the data obtained in the study. 

Intellectual tasks included writing, reading and calculation. The qualitative analysis established types of errors such as substitution or omission in writing and reading tasks that arose after feeling fatigue, and improvement in the execution with the aid of the evaluator (Table 3). In addition, about 50% of the sample did not consolidate literacy and calculation processes.

Table 3 Summary of the types of errors and execution in intellectual tasks. 

Source: Own elaboration based on the data obtained in the study.

Figure 2 presents examples of the writing task and the most representative error types in schoolchildren of different ages.

Figure 2 Examples of the most common types of typing errors. Source: Own elaboration based on the data obtained in the study. 

For the clinical characterization, the most frequent types of error and execution were identified in all sample individuals, which allowed obtaining a deficiency table of non-specific cerebral activation in

Mexican schoolchildren. Table 4 shows the frequency of the types of error committed in the different neuropsychological tasks.

Table 4 Most frequent types of error and execution in the sample throughout the neuropsychological evaluation. 

Source: Own elaboration based on the data obtained in the study.

In general, qualitative characteristics were observed during the execution of the proposed tasks. In most cases, children accepted the help provided by the evaluator to improve task execution; sometimes they requested help in order to repeat the instruction or to perform the task. A slow performance was observed during task execution, as well as distraction to various stimuli, restlessness or fatigue.

Furthermore, 10 types of errors were identified in more than 50% of the sample, mostly related to visual and auditory-verbal retention tasks, as well as graphic-perceptive, manual, verbal, reading and writing coordination tasks (Table 4).

It is important to specify that each child of the sample always presented more than one type of error in the tasks. For example, it was possible to see micrographs/macrographs in graphic tasks and instability in the trace (Figure 1).

Discussion

According to Fishman 33, several causes that affect the brain in the prenatal or postnatal period may delay the maturation of brain structures and their connections, which are manifested through the discordance between the social demands on the activity and the neurophysiological possibility to guarantee an adequate level of nonspecific regulation 10. In these cases, difficulties in development and school learning can be expected, based on the non-optimal functional state of the first brain unit according to Luria's conception 1,34.

The results showed that there are common clinical features in schoolchildren with functional deficits in the nonspecific brain activation factor during neuropsychological assessment, regardless of the initial reason for consultation (learning or behavior problems). Moreover, it was possible to identify sensitive tasks that help detecting this syndrome during the neuropsychological assessment based on instability, fatigue or latency during the assessment, as well as to differentiate these features from other types of errors related to functional weakness of other neuropsychological factors. In this work, such tasks were graphic-perceptive, retention in visual and auditory-verbal modality, manual coordination and writing.

The absence of these tasks in common psychometric evaluation protocols or quantitative cutoff tests, as well as the inability of specialists to detect the corresponding errors and differentiate them from other types of errors, makes it difficult to identify the state of nonspecific cerebral activation. These difficulties may be unnoticed or mistaken with problems in regulation and control factors, while

ADHD 35,36 or problems in executive functions 25 may be suspected from traditional diagnosis.

Data indicate latency and fatigue as the main characteristics, both in children with a previous ADHD diagnosis 13,14 and in cases in which a formal diagnosis is missing, only mentioning learning or behavior problems. Distraction before various stimuli and restlessness in the presence of fatigue was identified as a behavioral characteristic. In the literature, these behavioral characteristics are included under the term "hyperactivity" 35,36. In addition, it was observed that children accept the help of adults to improve their performance, as opposed to cases with primary affectation of other brain mechanisms in which the external assistance is not effective 9.

Results showed that executive instability during the evaluation allowed identifying insufficiency of the activation tone of the cerebral work. Therefore, in graphic-perceptive tasks, there is instability in the trace, as well as micrographs and macrographs. These types of errors have been identified in preschoolers and school children with ADHD in which functional weakness underlies the non-specific activation factor 8-10,13,14. Furthermore, Machinskaya & Semenova 20 identified difficulties in visual and auditory-verbal retention in schoolchildren aged between 7 and 8 years. These difficulties were related to information evocation without finding variability with age, which implies instability before the deficit 6.

It is important to note that results indicated that almost 50% of the children did not consolidate school skills, which may be related to the use of pedagogical strategies in primary school in general. At the same time, it can be assumed that the lack of nonspecific activation prevents evaluated children from properly consolidating school habits, turning them into children who manifest learning problems. The qualitative analysis of the types of errors presented by students allowed to identify phonemic substitutions that appeared due to tiredness during reading tasks (18 cases), as well as loss of horizontality and, sometimes, substitutions and omissions of consonants in writing tasks (15 cases). These characteristics imply that the deficit of this neuropsychological factor prevents adequate performance and the acquisition of the required abilities for literacy.

These data coincide with the latest publications that point out the need to consider the functionality of the first brain block during the neuropsychological evaluation and the presence of particular characteristics in the school activity, as well as the evidences during the neuropsychological evaluation. Future studies dedicated to the detailed analysis of errors in the literacy process will allow clarifying the errors related to the use of pedagogical strategies and cerebral functional weakness during the neuropsychological evaluation 37.

The obtained results may indicate a way for identifying difficulties related to the functional deficit of the first brain block, which has not been a frequent topic in neuropsychology so far. The possibility of correlating neuropsychological assessment data with electroencephalographic recordings can provide valuable information about the involvement of subcortical regulation levels at school age 38.

It is worth noting that qualitative neuropsychological evaluation and syndromic analysis allow the detection of objective clinical features. The first is a very useful tool for differential diagnosis and for the treatment of cases with learning problems and difficulties in development.

Conclusions

Qualitative neuropsychological assessment in Mexican schoolchildren provided relevant information for the detection of nonspecific cerebral activation syndrome. The syndrome was characterized by instability in the performance of neuropsychological tasks, fatigue, latency or short latencies at the beginning of an activity. Typical errors included micrographs/macrographs, loss of horizontality and instability in traces during graphical tasks, poor fluidity of the movements during motor tasks, as well as instability in the maintenance of the mnesic traces in visual and auditory-verbal retention tasks. The data obtained can be useful to generate intervention strategies and neuropsychological correction of children with this neuropsychological syndrome.

Acknowledgement

The authors express their gratitude to CONACYT for the granted postgraduate scholarship No. 377040, and to the Vice-rector of Research and Postgraduate Studies, and to BUAP for the support during the research which helped to finish the thesis project entitled " Neuropsychological syndrome: general cerebral activation deficit. Clinical characteristics in children."

References

1. Luria AR. El cerebro en acción. Barcelona: Roca; 1989. [ Links ]

2. Torteloro P, Vaninni G. Nuevos conceptos sobre la generación y el mantenimiento de la vigilia. Rev Neurol. 2010;50(12):747-58. [ Links ]

3. Peña-Casanova J. La neuropsicología y Vigotsky y Luria: El cerebro lesionado. Anuario de Psicología. 1985;33(2):29-42. [ Links ]

4. Cohelo-Rebelo-Maia LA, Fernández-sa Silva C, Ribeiro-Correia C, Perea-Bartolomé MV. El modelo de Alexander Romanovich Luria (Revisitado) y su aplicación a la evaluación neuropsicológica. Revista Galego-portuguesa dePsicoloxía eEducación. 2006;13(11-12):155-194. [ Links ]

5. Quintanar RL, Solovieva Y, Lázaro GE, Bonilla MR. Aproximación histórico-cultural: Fundamentos teórico-metodológicos. In: Eslava-Cobos J, Mejía L, Quintanar L, Solovieva Y, editors. Los trastornos de aprendizaje: perspectivas neuropsicológicas. Bogotá D.C.: Magisterio; 2008. p. 146-226. [ Links ]

6. Glozman JM. Developmental neuropsychology. London: Psychology-Taylor and Francis group, Press; 2013. [ Links ]

7. Portellano JA. Introducción a la neuropsicología. Madrid: McGrawHill; 2005. [ Links ]

8. Gómez MR. Características neuropsicológicas y electrofisiológicas en niños preescolares con Déficit de Atención e Hiperactividad [tesis de maestría]. Puebla: Facultad de Psicología, Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla; 2008. [ Links ]

9. Quintanar L, Gómez-Moya R, Solovieva Y, Bonilla-Sánchez MR. Características neuropsicológicas de niños preescolares con trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad. Revista CES Psicología. 2011;4(2):16-31. [ Links ]

10. Solovieva Y, Quintanar-Rojas L, Bonilla-Sánchez MR, Pelayo-González H. Neuropsicología y Electrofisiología del TDA en la edad preescolar. Puebla: Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla; 2009. [ Links ]

11. Machinskaya RI, Krupskaya EV. EEG Analysis of the Functional State of Deep Regulatory Structures of the Brain in Hyperactive Seven to Eight-Year-Old Children. Hum Physiol. 2001;27(3):368-70. [ Links ]

12. Machinskaya RI, Semenova OA, Absatova KA, Subogrova GA. Neuropsychological factors associated with cognitive deficits in Children with ADHD symptoms: EEG and neuropsychological analysis. Psychol & Neurosci. 2014;7(4):461-73. http://doi.org/bn38. [ Links ]

13. Morán-Paz GA. Análisis neuropsicológico y electroencefalográfico de niños escolares con TDA/TDAH [tesis]. Puebla: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Puebla; 2012. [ Links ]

14. Rivas-Zamudio X. Análisis neuropsicológico y electroencefalográfico de niños escolares de 4° y 6° grado con TDA/TDAH [tesis]. México: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Puebla; 2014. [ Links ]

15. Bezrukikh MM, Machinskaya R, Sugrobova GA. Differentiated Influence of the Functional Maturity of the Cortex and Brain Regulatory Structures on the Characteristics of Cognitive Activity in 7-8-Year-Old Children. Hum Physiol. 1999;25(5):510-7. [ Links ]

16. Lukashevich IP, Machinskaya RI, Fishman MN. Determination of brain function in young schoolchildren with learning problems. Human Physiology. 1994;20(5):353-8. [ Links ]

17. Machinskaya RI. Brain organization of voluntary selective attention in first grade children with learning difficulties. In: Taddei-Frretti, Musio C, editors. Neuronal Bases and Psychological Aspects of Consciousness. London: World Scientific Publishing; 1999. 343-7. [ Links ]

18. Machinskaya RI, Lukashevich IP, Fishman MN. Dynamics of Brain Electrical Activity in 5-8-Year-Old Normal Children and children with learning difficulties. Hum Physiol. 1997;23(5):517-22. [ Links ]

19. Machinskaya RI, Subogrova GA, Semenova OA. An interdisciplinary approach to analysis of the cerebral mechanisms of learning difficulties in children. Experience of studies of children with sings of ADHD. Neurosci behavphysiol. 2015;45(1):58-73. http://doi.org/b5ds. [ Links ]

20. Machinskaya RI, Semenova O. Peculiarities of Formation of the Cognitive Functions in Junior School Children with Different Maturity of Regulatory Brain Systems. JEvolBiochem Phys. 2004;40(5):528-38. [ Links ]

21. Machinskaya RI, Sokolova LS, Krupskaya EV. Formation of the functional organization ofthe cerebral cortex at rest in young schoolchildren varying in the maturity of cerebral regulatory systems: II. Analysis of EEG a-rhythm coherence. Hum Physiol. 2007;33(2):129-38. http://doi.org/bbgwn2. [ Links ]

22. Barroso-Martín JM, León-Carrión J. Funciones ejecutivas: control, planificación y organización del conocimiento. Rev. de Psicol. Gral y Aplic. 2002;55(1):27-44. [ Links ]

23. Akhutina T. Neuropsicología de la edad escolar. Una aproximación histórico-cultural. Acta Neurol Colomb. 2008;24:S17-S30. [ Links ]

24. Xomskaya E. El problema de los factores en la neuropsicología. Revista Española de Neurpsicología. 2002;4(2-3):151-67. [ Links ]

25. Solovieva Y, Quintanar L. Syndromic analysis of ADHD at preschool age according to A. R. Luria concept. Psychol & Neurosci. 2014;7(4):443-52. http://doi.org/b5dt. [ Links ]

26. Luria A. Las funciones corticales superiores del hombre. México D.F.: Fontamara. 1986. [ Links ]

27. Shevchenko I, Glozman J. ADHD in children: Mechanism and remediation. The Open Behavioral Science Journal. 2015;9(Supl 1-M5):32-9. http://doi.org/b5dv. [ Links ]

28. Korsakova NK, Moskovichute LI. Estructuras cerebrales subcorticales y los procesos psíquicos. Moscú: Universidad Estatal de Moscú; 1985. [ Links ]

29. Xomskaya ED. Neuropsicología. Moscú: Universidad Estatal de Moscú ; 1987. [ Links ]

30. Luria A. El cerebro humano y los procesos psíquicos. Análisis neuropsicológico de la actividad consciente. Barcelona: Fontanela; 1979. [ Links ]

31. Solovieva Y, Quintanar L, León-Carrión J. Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil "Puebla-Sevilla".Puebla: Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla; en prensa 2013. [ Links ]

32. Solovieva Y, Quintanar L. Verificación del Éxito Escolar en la escuela Primaria. Puebla: Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla ; 2012. [ Links ]

33. Fishman MN. The functional state ofthe Cortex and brainstem regulatory structures in children with speech development disorders. Hum Physiol. 2001;27(5):535-8. http://doi.org/bs3k4c. [ Links ]

34. Manga D, Ramos F. El legado de Luria y la neuropsicología escolar. Psy, Soc, & Educ. 2011;3(1):1-13. [ Links ]

35. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-4). Washington D.C.: APS; 2000. [ Links ]

36. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Washington D.C.: APS ; 2013. [ Links ]

37. Akhutina TV, Korneev AA, Matveeva EY, Agris AR. Age-related changes of higher mental functions in 7-9 years old children with different types of state regulation deficits. Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics. 2015;12(3):131-52. [ Links ]

38. Solovieva Y, Pelayo-González H, Méndez-Balbuena I, Machinskaya R, Morán G. Correlación de análisis neuropsicológico y electroencefalográfico en escolares con diagnóstico de TDA. eNeurobiología. 2016;7(15):1-15. [ Links ]

1Luna-Villanueva B, Solovieva Y, Lázaro-García E, Quintanar L. Clinical features of brain activation deficit in children. Rev. Fac. Med. 2017;65(3):417-23. English. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/revfacmed.v65n3.57057.

2Luna-Villanueva B, Solovieva Y, Lázaro-García E, Quintanar L. [Características clínicas de niños con déficit de activación cerebral general]. Rev. Fac. Med. 2017;65(2):417-23. English. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.15446/revfacmed.v65n3.57057.

Funding This research was supported by Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACYT) through scholarship No. 377040.

Received: April 19, 2016; Accepted: July 11, 2016

*Corresponding author: Yulia Solovieva. 3 oriente 403, Centro Histórico. Phone number: +52 2222425370. Puebla. México. Email: yulia.solovieva@correo.buap.mx.

Conflict of interests

None declared by the authors.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License