SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.51 issue2Induction of Affective States with Pictures: Second Colombian Validation of the International Affective Pictures System (IAPS)The Fiftieth Anniversary of the Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología: A Bibliometric Analysis author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología

Print version ISSN 0120-0534

rev.latinoam.psicol. vol.51 no.2 Bogotá May/Aug. 2019

https://doi.org/10.14349/rlp.2019.v51.n2.6 

Articles

Adaptation and Validation of the Economic and Financial Literacy Test for Chilean secondary students

Adaptación y validación del Test de Alfabetización Económica y Financiera en estudiantes secundarios chilenos

Marianela Denegri-Coriaa  * 

Andrés Concha-Salgadob 

Jocelyne Sepúlveda-Aravenaa 

a Centro de Excelencia en Psicología y del Consumo, Núcleo en Ciencias Sociales y Humanidades, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile.

b Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile


Abstract

This article presents the results of the analysis of psychometric properties of the Economic and Financial Literacy Test (TAEF-E) after its application in 811 high school students from nine educational establishments in the cities of Santiago (48.6%) and Temuco (51.4%) and whose sample had a 52.5% participation of women. For the study, three scales were applied: TAEF-E, the Scale of Susceptibility to the influence of peers in consumption and the Attitude Scale towards materialism for adolescents, managing to demonstrate optimal levels of internal consistency, adequate factor validity with a one-dimensional structure and significant correlations with sociodemographic and attitudinal variables, which allow to ratify as a whole, the validity and reliability of TAEF-E as an instrument to measure economic and financial literacy in secondary school students.

Keywords: Economic Literacy; Financial Reasoning; Adolescents; Validity; Reliability

Resumen

Este artículo presenta los resultados del análisis de propiedades psicométricas del Test de Alfabetización Económica y Financiera (TAEF-E) tras su aplicación en 811 estudiantes de secundaria de nueve establecimientos educativos de las ciudades de Santiago (48.6%) y Temuco (51.4%) y cuya muestra tuvo una participación del 52.5% de mujeres. Para el estudio, se aplicaron tres escalas: El TAEF-E, la Escala de Susceptibilidad a la influencia de los pares en el consumo y la Escala de Actitud hacia el materialismo para adolescentes, logrando demostrar niveles óptimos de consistencia interna, adecuada validez factorial con una estructura unidimensional y correlaciones significativas con variables sociodemográficas y actitudinales, que permiten ratificar en su conjunto, la validez y confiabilidad del TAEF-E como instrumento para medir la alfabetización económica y financiera en estudiantes de secundaria.

Palabras clave: Alfabetización económica; razonamiento financiero; adolescentes; validez; confiabilidad

The concept of economic and financial literacy (EFL), understood as the ability to understand the social economic model systematically, to involve and apply basic economic concepts and interpret events and economic policies which are not always explicit (Denegri & Delval, 2002). It also refers to the ability to make informed economic judgments and effective personal, family, and professional decisions (Cohen & Candace, 2011; Rogers, 2014). Based on this perspective, it includes the development of an individual's ability to act in the economic and financial world in relation to their experience, knowledge, and abilities. Additionally, the concept refers to a social subject that places individuals in more favourable or unfavourable positions regarding their development (Bay, Catasús, & Johed, 2014).

For this reason, it is key for today's societies to evaluate EFL levels to explore ways they can be improved (Huston, 2010). Moreover, it is also an attractive resource for researchers in the field of economic socialization (Gunter & Furnham, 1998), particularly for adolescents as many attitudes and consumption habits are established between the ages of ten and 15 that will continue into adulthood (Denegri & Delval, 2002; Ozgen, 2003).

For children and adolescents, measuring economic and financial literacy is particularly important (Gempp, et al., 2006) because their developmental characteristics actively construct explanations about the economic world that are anchored to the cognitive resources available and based on their developmental level (Delval, Enesco, & Navarro, 1994; Denegri, Delval, Ripoll, Palavecinos, & Keller, 1998). Therefore, the focus of evaluating EFL in children and adolescents cannot be on the content or management of specific information but rather on economic reasoning, i.e., the explanations and cognitive maps that the child uses to represent the economic world (Denegri, 1995).

In the case of Chile, the results of the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2015 show students' low performances. Also, the highest concentrations are found in the low performance levels, and the socioeconomic gap is clear since students with more economic resources have, on average, better scores than the most vulnerable young people (Agencia de Calidad de la Educación, 2017).

Chile has had experience in this area from 2004 when the Test of Economic Literacy in Children (TEL-C) (Cortés, Quezada, & Sepúlveda, 2004; Gempp, et al., 2006) was created and validated for children and adolescents between the ages of ten and 14, which was based on the Psychogene-sis of Economic Thinking model proposed by Denegri (1995) and validated qualitatively in several subsequent investigations (Amar, Llanos, Abello, & Denegri, 2003; Bonifacio de Araujo, 2009; Denegri & Delval, 2002; Denegri, et al., 1998).

This theoretical model considers Piaget bases and describes the development of economic thinking through progressive levels of increasing complexity that express a progression in the ability to understand situations and solve problems associated with the representation of the economic world. This model posits the existence of three developmental levels of economic thinking: (a) Level I (5 to 9 years old), further divided into: sublevel Ia (Extraeconomic Thinking) and sub-level Ib (Primitive Economic Thinking); (b) Level II (> 10 years old, adolescents, and even some adults): Subordinate Economic Thinking, and (c) Level III (older adolescents and adults): Independent Economic Thinking (Denegri, 1995; 1998). The TEL-C, therefore, has a graduated response format that is coherent with this theoretical background, which provides two ways of correcting the responses: one with three points that maintains sublevel Ia and level III but groups sublevel Ib with level II, a solution that presented better psychometric behaviour (Gempp, et al., 2006), and another with four points in which each alternative corresponds to one of the four levels and sublevels of the economic reasoning model.

Despite this advance, there is no similar instrument that can be used with adolescents over the age of 14 and that fully covers this developmental stage. The EFLT-S was, thus, based on the TEL-C and also from the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development's guidelines (OECD) (2012), which explains that financial literacy among young people should include: competencies in managing money to deal with bank accounts and credit/debit cards, how to plan and manage personal finances, understanding saving mechanisms, indebtedness and taxes, anticipating risk and evaluating rewards, and more specific aspects relating to consumer's rights and responsibilities.

Previous studies show that differences in financial literacy levels, are related to socioeconomic status (the higher the status, the greater the literacy), educational level (the higher the education level, the greater the literacy), sex (men score better on EFL evaluations), and age (adults have better literacy than young people and seniors) (Domínguez, 2015; Hastings, Madrian, & Skimmyhorn, 2013; Webley & Nyhus, 2013).

On a psychological level, low financial competency is associated with other economic behaviours including debt accumulation, high-cost loans (Lusardi & Tufano, 2015), poor mortgage choice, and delinquent mortgages (Gerardi, Goette, & Meier, 2010; Moore, 2003). On the other hand, materialism correlates with impulse buying and indebtedness (Troisi, Christopher, & Marek, 2006), and, in the case of adolescents, it is associated with consumer susceptibility to interpersonal influence (Cárdenas, 2017; Kretschmer & Pike, 2010).

This background emphasizes the need to move forward to assess economic and financial literacy levels in adolescents because they are facing more and more complex financial products and services and a market that has identified them as a segment to address their aggressive marketing strategies (Marshall, 2014; OECD, 2012). Considering these challenges, reliable and valid measurements must be developed to determine their levels of economic literacy and financial education strategies must be proposed based on the results. For this reason, the objectives of this study were: (1) to adapt linguistically and developmentally, (2) to determine the best way to correct the items, (3) to determine the factor structure, (4) to determine the internal consistency, and 85) to correlate the total test scores with theoretically compatible variables.

Method

Participants

The autors used a two-stage sampling: the first was non-probabilistic and purposive, and a list of urban, co-ed high schools was compiled. In the second stage, we used a probability sample with a 5% error rate and a 95% confidence level. It was organized in a 3x2x2 design and considered the following control variables: type of school (municipal, subsidized, and private), geographic zone identified by commune (centre and south of the country), and sex (male and female). Eight hundred and eleven secondary students from nine schools in Santiago (48.6%) and Temuco (51.4%) participated. Fifty-two-point-five percent were female, with an average age of 15.05 (SD = 0.858), and 47.5% were male, with a mean age of 14.92 (SD = 0.961). Table 1 summarizes the characteristics according to the participants' age, sex, and socioeconomic level. It can be seen that the highest frequencies are for the ages between 13 and 15 and for the middle socioeconomic strata.

Table 1 Sample characteristics according to age, sex, and socioeconomic status. 

Note: Due to missing values, only those cases that have complete information in these three variables are shown.

Source: own construction.

Instruments

We used a questionnaire to ascertain personal variables such as sex and age, socio-family variables including Family Socioeconomic Level (obtained from the education level and occupational category of the primary household wage earner) and socio-educational variables including the School Vulnerability Index (classification and hierarchical structuring of schools according to the percentage of students considered vulnerable based on their socioeconomic condition). Information from both these categories was used for the analyses. Additionally, we applied three instruments:

Economic and Financial Literacy Test for Secondary students (EFLT-S)

The EFLT-S contained 27 items in its original design. The response options intentionally include four quality levels: the respondent chooses an option which is scored between one and four and is in line with the four sublevels of the economic thinking model (Denegri, 1998). The following is an example: 9. We have to pay taxes every month, why?, and the possible responses are shown in decreasing order: (a) Because we must contribute to financing works and services provided by the State (4-point response); (d) Because they are discounted from paychecks every month (3-point response); (c) Because we must pay them through electricity and water bills (2-point response); and (b) Because it is mandatory (1-point response). The total scores are obtained by adding the points from the items: the higher the score, the higher the respondent's level of economic and financial reasoning.

Measure of Consumer Susceptibility to Interpersonal Influence.

The instrument was originally developed by Bearden, Netemeyer, and Teel (1989) and adapted in a reduced version for adolescents by Zhang (2001). This is composed of 8 items and uses a Likert scale from 1 to 6 that measures susceptibility to peers' normative influence, which can be translated into approval, image imposed by the group, and a sense of belonging achieved by consuming certain products or services. The following is an example: 4. In order to maintain a good relationship with my friends, I often buy the same products or brands as they do. In this measurement, the scale presented adequate internal consistency: McDonald's Omega = 0.923 CI 95% [0.899 0.940] and Ordinal Alpha = 0.923 CI 95°% [0.899 0.940].

Youth Materialism Scale

This scale for adolescents was designed by Goldberg, Gorn, Peracchio, and Bamossy (2003), and the ten items on the Likert scale (from one to six) reflect different materialist values. One question on the instrument is: 2. I would be happier if I had more money to buy more things for myself. In the present measurement, the instrument presented adequate internal consistency: McDonald's Omega = 0.923CI 95% [0.899 0.940] and Ordinal Alpha = 0.923 CI 95% [0.899 0.940].

Procedure

After receiving authorization from the school authorities, the instruments were applied in schools in the respective classrooms. The application was collective, anonymous, and participation was voluntary. Informed consent was given to the parents to sign if they authorized their child's participation in the study; once this document was received, students were then asked to indicate their intention to participate by signing another informed consent.

Analytic Strategy

Prior to performing the analyses, we explored the database and considered the 27 items on the test. Also, the following decisions were made: (a) forty subjects with two or more pieces of missing data were eliminated (> 5%): a total of 811 cases; (b) the missing data for those students was attributed to the need for an observation to be omitted using the expectation-maximization method (Dempster, Laird, & Rubin 1977); (c) a sample of 200 cases was selected at random to perform the exploratory factor analysis; d) the remaining 611 cases were kept in a separate database to perform the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA).

In CFA, as an estimator, we used the Means and Variance Adjusted Unweighted Least Squares (ULSMV) method on a polychoric matrix. The fit of the model data was assessed using Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). Acceptable levels for optimum fit were CFI and TLI > .95, and RMSEA < .05. Acceptable levels for reasonable fit were CFI and TLI > .90, and RMSEA < .08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2005; Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). We also used Schwarz's Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) to compare the models (a lower value indicates better fit).

We analyzed the data using the FACTOR program for the exploratory factor analysis, Stata to compare the parsimonious fit indices of the models, Mplus for the confirmatory factor analysis, and IBM SPSS Statistics for the rest of the descriptive and inferential analyses.

Results

Adaptation

We conducted the adaptation of the EFLT-S in the following stage: 1) identification of the relevant items of the TEL-C (Gempp, et al., 2006), 2) language adjustments in the selected items for the adolescent population, and 3) incorporation of new areas of financial literacy that the OECD suggests (OECD, 2014).

Once the items were reviewed and adapted, we evaluated the validity of the test content using three expert judges involved in the following academic and research fields: Financial Education, Economics, and Economic Psychology (Escobar-Pérez & Cuervo-Martínez, 2008). As a result, the overall agreement percentage of theoretical coherence was 82.72%, and the Free-marginal Kappa was 0.77 95% CI [0.62, 0.92], which showed substantial agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977). Twenty-seven items were obtained, validated in terms of content; modifications were mainly made to the vocabulary, and two new items were incorporated.

Determining the form of correction

We tested confirmatory factor models with three and four-point corrections in order to find the most parsimonious model (fewer items) with the best fit. Table 2 contains the summary of the comparisons between models; the one with 21 items that had the 4-point correction was the best, We, therefore, recommend this solution. This option was chosen to have a lower Akaike information criterion than the others, which is recommended when choosing a model.

Table 2 Comparison between model's parsimony criteria to determine the best correction. 

Note 1: in the 3-point correction, the first analysis suggested 5 items be deleted.

Note 2: in the 4-point correction, the first analysis suggested 6 items be deleted.

Factor structure

First of all, work was undertaken with a random sample of two-hundred cases to perform an exploratory factor analysis in the FACTOR programme (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2006) using polychoric correlation matrices since the test items are measured at ordinal level. In addition, we used parallel analysis (Timmerman & Lorenzo-Seva, 2011) and robust unweighted least squares.

The sample adequacy measurements permitted factorization, KMO = 0.773 (acceptable), and there was a significant result for Bartlett's tests of sphericity (x2 = 1113, df = 351, p < .001). The analysis yielded a unifactorial solution for the 27 items, which explained 26.592% of the total variance. This analysis served to identify the unidimensional structure of the test.

Once the unidimensionality of the test was determined, two confirmatory models were evaluated: the first with the 27 original items and the second with only the 21 items that had factor weights greater than 0.3. Six test items were finally eliminated that did not fulfil the criterion. The results indicated that the best unifactorial model is the one with 21 items, which explained 31.5% of the total variance with suitable indices of fit (see Table 3).

Table 3 Descriptive statistics of the items and results from the exploratory, confirmatory, and reliability analyses. 

Note 1: asterisks show those items that have factor loadings less than or equal to 0.3 (removed from the second model).

Note 2: n= number of participants; k= number of items; CFI= Comparative Fit Index; TLI= Tucker Lewis Index.

Internal consistency

Following recommendations from Trizano-Hermosilla and Alvarado (2016), we used McDonald's coefficient omega due to the presence of a pattern of asymmetrical responses in the test items (see Table 4) in addition to the ordinal alpha, which is suggested for items with ordinal measurement. In the various analyses, coefficients were over 0.85, which is interpreted as acceptable in terms of reliability. In the final model, McDonald's Omega was 0.886, 95% CI [0.819 0.926], and Ordinal Alpha was 0.885, 95% CI [0.815 0.924]. Also, the specific analysis of each item based on the ordinal alpha indicated that it was not necessary to eliminate more items to increase the reliability of the test (see Table 3).

Table 4 Category percentages and descriptives for each item (n=811). 

Correlation between variables

We correlated the scores with sociodemographic, attitudinal, socioeconomic, and socio-educational variables to evaluate the external validity of the instrument. The results obtained relating to sex show that the women had higher scores in economic and financial literacy t(764) = 2.679, p = .008, d = 0.194: the effect size of this comparison was small. With respect to the attitudinal variables, the scores on the EFL showed small statistically significant correlations in the expected direction with Materialism r(808) = -.170, p < .001 and with Susceptibility to interpersonal influence r(808) = -.209, p < .001. In relation to the two last variables, economic and financial literacy presented medium statistically significant correlations according to what was theoretically expected with the Vulnerability School Index r(810) = -.467, p < .001 and with Home Socioeconomic Status r(774) = .339, p < .001. Table 5 summarizes this information.

Table 5 Correlations between EFLT-S score and theory-guided variables. 

Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; df = degrees of freedom; n = sample size; BCa 95% CI = bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap 95% confidence interval.

Discussion

In this section, the scope of the results is discussed, and we consider the objectives set out in this study. First, the psychometric properties of the EFLT-S were studied, which provided a new instrument to assess adolescents' economic and financial literacy. In addition, each of the specific objectives was fulfilled, which permits us to establish the following lines of discussion.

In relation to the adaptation of the EFLT-S, we followed a procedure that could guarantee the content validity of the instrument. This was undertaken considering the theoretical model as the reference and then checking the item proposals with expert judges.

Once the results of the form of correction were established, although both alternatives produced similar results, the decision was made to use the one with four options that had fewer items, better indices of parsimony, and greater response variability in each item; this respected the Psychogenesis of Economic Thinking model (Denegri, 1995). A possible future challenge could be to apply a new analysis with a greater age range under the Item Response Theory (IRT) framework to determine which of the two alternatives yields results that present better psychometric behaviour. This procedure has been used in several previous studies such as Martínez-León, Mathes, Avendaño, Peña, and Sierra (2018), García-Cueto et al. (2015), and Suárez-Álvarez, Pedrosa, García-Cueto and Muñiz (2016).

The factor structure of the test is clearly unidimensional with adequate saturations in the single factor and adequate indices of fit. These results contribute evidence in favour of the (factor) construct validity, indicating an underlying construct that is common to the 21 test items. This is also theoretically consistent given the purpose of the test is to measure the economic reasoning exemplified in the explanations and cognitive maps that the child uses to represent the economic world (Denegri, 1995).

With respect to the internal consistency, the test results revealed optimal indicators considering coefficients for ordinal items, which means that the items consistently measure the EFL construct.

Finally, the analyses of the correlations indicate that the results of four of the five variables were in line with what was theoretically and empirically expected. In the case of materialism, those subjects who have more elaborate economic reasoning tend to present more unfavourable attitudes to the variable, which is consistent with other studies (Troisi, Christopher, & Marek, 2006). The result about susceptibility to interpersonal influence reveals that those young people who obtain higher test scores are less susceptible to peer influence in the area of consumption and buying, which has been indicated in previous studies (Cárdenas, 2017; Kretschmer & Pike, 2010). Although the effect size of the correlations is small, the EFLT-S manages to identify relationships in the expected direction.

The results of the socio-educational and socioeconomic variables showed that economically disadvantaged families and schools with higher vulnerability rates achieved lower levels of economic reasoning. This finding is consistent with previous studies (Atkinson, McKay, Kempson, & Collard, 2006; Domínguez, 2015; Hastings, Madrian, & Skimmyhorn, 2013).

The previously described correlational results may be considered evidence in favour of the concurrent validity of the test, which is also a form of external validity (Elosua, 2003).

With respect to the sex variable, the results indicate significant differences of low magnitude: females obtain higher levels of economic reasoning, which contradicts previous findings (Lusardi & Mitchell, 2011; Lusardi & Tufano, 2015). However, there are mixed findings: some studies indicate that differences between men and women do not exist (Agencia de Calidad de la Educación, 2017; Agnew & Harrison, 2015; Davies, Mangan, & Telhaj, 2005), but other results show that girls obtain better scores than boys (Denegri & Sepúlveda, 2014).

A possible limitation of the study includes the age homogeneity of the sample since 70% of the participants were between 14 and 15 years of age; therefore, there is a need to extend the age ranges in future research.

Another weakness of the study is the low percentage of variance explained in the Exploratory Factor Analysis (26.59% with sample 1 and then 31.5% with sample 2); however, this situation is countervailed by good indexes of adjustment in the Confirmatory Factor Analysis.

A final limitation is the small "effect size" observed in the correlations: an aspect that should continue to be reviewed in future investigations with diverse populations.

In conclusion, we considered the main contribution of this study to be that the psychometric properties of the test were evaluated, which provides a valid and reliable instrument to measure economic and financial literacy in secondary students: a specific population for which there is no instrument in this area. It provides a tool to diagnose and evaluate this variable in the context of interventions in the field of economic and financial literacy for a segment of the population that is vulnerable due to their developmental characteristics and market pressure when there is scant financial education (Davies, Howie, Mangan, & Shqiponja, 2002; Lusardi & Tufano, 2015).

People's economic education refers to the improvement of economic decision-making processes as they develop competencies to effectively solve problems. In this educational process, formal and informal financial education plays a relevant role. The family is of particular importance as an agent of socialization that influences adult consumers' behaviour (Sandoval-Escobar, Pineda-Marín, & Ávila-Campos, 2018).

References

Agencia de Calidad de la Educación. (2017). PISA Educación Financiera: 1 de cada 3 estudiantes aprende a manejar su dinero en la escuela. Santiago: Agencia Educación. [ Links ]

Agnew, S., & Harrison, N. (2015). Financial literacy and student attitudes to debt: A cross national study examining the influence of gender on personal finance concepts. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 25, 122-129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2015.04.006. [ Links ]

Amar, J., Llanos, M., Abello, R., & Denegri, M. (2003). Desarrollo del pensamiento económico en niños de la región caribe colombiana. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 35(1), 7-18. [ Links ]

Atkinson, A., McKay, S., Kempson, E., & Collard, S. (2006). Levels of Financial Capability in the UK: Results of a Baseline Survey. Consumer Research, 47. [ Links ]

Bay, C., Catasús, B., & Johed, G. (2014). Situating financial literacy. Critical Perspectives on Accounting, 25(1), 36-45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpa.2012.11.011. [ Links ]

Bearden, W., Netemeyer, R., & Teel, J. (1989). Measurement of Consumer Susceptibility to Interpersonal Influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), 473-481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209186. [ Links ]

Bonifacio de Araujo, R. (2009). Alfabetizacao Economica Comprom-isso Social na Educacao das Criancas. Brasil: Editorial Metodista. [ Links ]

Cárdenas, V. (2017). Prácticas de Consumo y uso del dinero, Valores Materiales y Susceptibilidad a la Influencia de Pares en Adolescentes Escolarizados de Temuco. (Thesis in Psychology). Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile. [ Links ]

Cohen, M., & Candace, N. (November 2011). Financial Literacy: A Step for Clients towards Financial Inclusion. Commissioned Workshop Paper presented in the Global Microcredit Summit, November 14-17. Valladolid. [ Links ]

Cortés, L., Quezada, M., & Sepúlveda, J. (2004). Construcción de un Test de Alfabetización Económica para Niños (TAE-N). (Thesis in Psychology). Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile. [ Links ]

Davies, P., Howie, H., Mangan, J., & Shqiponja, T. (2002). Economic aspects of citizenship education: An investigation of students understanding. The Curriculum Journal, 13(2), 201-223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585170210136859. [ Links ]

Davies, P., Mangan, J., & Telhaj, S. (2005). Bold, reckless and adaptable? Explaining gender differences in economic thinking and attitudes. British Educational Research Journal, 31(1), 29-48. [ Links ]

Delval, J., Enesco, I., & Navarro, A. (1994). La construcción del conocimiento económico. In M. Rodrigo, Contexto y Desarrollo Social (345-383). Madrid: Síntesis Psicología. [ Links ]

Dempster, A., Laird, N., & Rubin, D. (1977). Likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodological), 39(1), 1-38. [ Links ]

Denegri, M. (1995). El desarrollo de las ideas acerca del origen y circulación del dinero: un estudio evolutivo con niños y adolescentes. Madrid: Ediciones Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. [ Links ]

Denegri, M. (1998). La construcción de nociones económicas en la infancia y adolescencia. In J. Ferro & J. Amar, Desarrollo humano, perspectiva para el Siglo XXI (203-219). Colombia: Ediciones UNINORTE. [ Links ]

Denegri, M., & Delval, J. (2002). Concepciones evolutivas acerca de la fabricación del dinero II: Los tipos de respuestas. Investigación en la escuela. Revista de Investigación e Innovación Escolar, 48, 55-70. [ Links ]

Denegri, M., & Sepúlveda, J. (2014). Evaluación de un programa de educación económica, "Yo y la Economía", en escolares chilenos de Educación General Básica. LIBERABIT, 20(1), 175-186. [ Links ]

Denegri, M., Delval, J., Ripoll, M., Palavecinos, M., & Keller, A. (1998). Desarrollo del pensamiento económico en la infancia y adolescencia. Boletín de Investigación Educacional, 13, 291-308. [ Links ]

Domínguez, J. (2015). El Informe PISA y la educación financiera: la primera hornada. eXtoikos, 17, 43-45. [ Links ]

Elosua, P. (2003). Sobre la validez de los tests. Psicothema, 15(2), 315-321. [ Links ]

Escobar-Pérez, J., & Cuervo-Martínez, A. (2008). Validez de contenido y juicio de expertos: una aproximación a su utilización. Avances en Medición, 6, 27-36. [ Links ]

García-Cueto, E., Rodríguez-Díaz, F. J., Bringas-Molleda, C., López-Cepero, J., Paíno-Quesada, S., & Rodríguez-Franco, L. (2015). Development of the gender role attitudes scale (GRAS) amongst young Spanish people. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 15(1), 61-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2014.10.004. [ Links ]

Gempp, R., Denegri, M., Caprile, C., Cortés, L., Quezada, M., & Sepúlveda, J. (2006). Medición de la Alfabetización Económica en Niños: Oportunidades Diagnósticas con el Modelo de Crédito Parcial. Psykhe, 15(1), 13-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718-22282006000100002. [ Links ]

Gerardi, K., Goette, L., & Meier, S. (2010). Financial literacy and sub-prime mortgage delinquency: evidence from a survey matched to administrative data. Atlanta: Fed. Reserve Bank Atlanta. [ Links ]

Goldberg, M., Gorn, G., Peracchio, L., & Bamossy, G. (2003). Understanding materialism among youth. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(3), 278-288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1303_09. [ Links ]

Gunter, F., & Furnham, A. (1998). Children as Consumer. A psychological analysis of the young people's market. London: Routledge. [ Links ]

Hastings, J., Madrian, B., & Skimmyhorn, W. (2013). Financial Literacy, financial education and economic outcomes. Annual Review of Economic, 1(5), 347-373. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-economics-082312-125807. [ Links ]

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. [ Links ]

Huston, S. (2010). Measuring Financial Literacy. The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 44(2), 296-316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.2010.01170.x. [ Links ]

Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press. [ Links ]

Kretschmer, T., & Pike, A. (2010). Links between nonshared friendship experiences and adolescent siblings' differences in aspirations. Journal of Adolescence, 33, 101-110. [ Links ]

Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2529310. [ Links ]

Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. (2006). FACTOR: A computer program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behavior Research Methods, 38(1), 88-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03192753. [ Links ]

Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. (2011). Financial Literacy around the World: An Overview. Discussion Paper 02/2011-023. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1810551. [ Links ]

Lusardi, A., & Tufano, P. (2015). Debt literacy, financial experiences, and overindebtedness. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 14(4), 332-368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747215000232. [ Links ]

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In Search of Golden Rules: Comment on Hypothesis-Testing Approaches to Setting Cutoff Values for Fit Indexes and Dangers in Overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) Findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320-341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2. [ Links ]

Martínez-León, N. C., Mathes, E., Avendaño, B. L., Peña, J. J., & Sierra, J. C. (2018). Psychometric Study of the Interpersonal Jealousy Scale in Colombian Samples. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 50(1), 21-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.14349/rlp.2018.v50.n1.3. [ Links ]

Marshall, E. (May 2014). El desafío de la Educación Financiera. Presentación efectuada en el marco de una actividad organizada por la Fundación Coopeuch. May 28. Santiago. [ Links ]

Moore, D. (2003). Survey of financial literacy in Washington State: knowledge, behavior, attitudes, and experiences. Technical Report, 3-39. Social and Economic Sciences Research Center, Washington State University. http://dx.doi.org/10.13140/2.1.4729.4722. [ Links ]

OECD. (2012). Pisa 2012 Financial Literacy Assessment Framework, Abril 2012. Paris: OECD Publishing. [ Links ]

OECD. (2014). PISA 2012 Results: Students and Money: Financial Literacy Skills for the 21st Century (Volume VI). Paris: OECD Publishing . [ Links ]

Ozgen, O. (2003). An analysis of child consumers in Turkey. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 27, 366-380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1470-6431.2003.00306.x. [ Links ]

Rogers, J. (2014). The Need for Economic Literacy for Students and Teachers: Insights from John Dewey. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 14(1), 419-422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/asap.12050. [ Links ]

Sandoval-Escobar, M., Pineda-Marín, C. & Ávila-Campos, J. (2018). Barreras para la socialización económica de las familias y estrategias empleadas para la educación económica y del consumo en los niños. In E. D. Forero, (Ed.). Elección, razonamiento y decisión, 11-48. Bogotá: Konrad Lorenz Editores. [ Links ]

Suárez-Álvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., García-Cueto, E., & Muñiz, J. (2016). Locus of Control revisited: development of a new bi-dimensional measure. Anales de Psicología, 32(2), 578-586. http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.32.2.200781. [ Links ]

Timmerman, M., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality Assessment of Ordered Polytomous Items with Parallel Analysis. Psychological Methods, 16, 209-220. http:dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023353. [ Links ]

Trizano-Hermosilla, I., & Alvarado, J. (2016). Best Alternatives to Cronbach's Alpha Reliability in Realistic Conditions: Congeneric and Asymmetrical Measurements. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 769. http:dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00769. [ Links ]

Troisi, J., Christopher, A., & Marek, P. (2006). Materialism and money spending disposition as predictors of economic and personality variables. North American Journal of Psychology, 8(3), 421-436. [ Links ]

Webley, P., & Nyhus, E. (2013). Economic socialization, saving and assets in European young adults. Economics of Education Review, 33, 19-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2012.09.001. [ Links ]

Zhang, M. (2001). Differences in susceptibility to interpersonal influence - Chinese s-generation adolescents and older adults. (Unpublished Thesis). University of Guelph, Ontario, Canada. [ Links ]

Appendix 1. Economic and Financial Literacy Test for Secondary students (EFLT-S).

  1. There is concern because everything is increasing in price, and you are asked what could be done to control this? What is you answer?

  2. How do you decide how much money should be issued (printed) in an economy such as the Chilean one?

  3. The owner of a company is very worried because his company is having liquidity problems. What measures could the employer take to avoid bankruptcy?

  4. The owner of a store has to buy bread to sell to his customers, how much should he sell it for?

  5. In a store located in the centre of the city, a shirt costs 15 thousand pesos, and in a store located far from downtown, the same shirt costs 10 thousand pesos. Why does this price difference occur if it is the same shirt?

  6. How do banks obtain economic benefits?

  7. People must pay taxes every month, why?

  8. A worker believes that they do not pay enough for his work. What do you think he should do?

  9. Traditionally more fish is eaten than red meat at Easter, and fish prices rise. What family measures can be taken to address the price increase?

  10. One person bought a piece of land fifty years ago, and it cost $ 200,000 pesos. They are now thinking of selling it but do not know how much to charge for it. Do you think that the land is worth the same as it was fifty years ago?

  11. Why do you have to have cash to buy what is needed?

  12. On the news they say that the cost of living has gone up. Why does this happen?

  13. What happens if the amount of money in circulation increases?

  14. When you buy chocolate, you are paying: (Students must select the correct alternative).

  15. Your family has been saving to buy their own house, but Chile has just qualified for the soccer world cup in a neighbouring country. Your family is thinking of using the house savings to travel and see the national team play. What should they do?

  16. Your neighbour wants to open a bakery in your neighbourhood, but there are already two on the same block. What would you recommend?

  17. A family that already has its own house in an affluent neighbourhood inherited a house that is located in a downtown and well-off part of the city. What should the family do with that house?

  18. The main source of income for a family is a taxi cab driven by the father. They are thinking about insuring the car to protect the business. What would you recommend?

  19. Two friends are going to rent a house. They have been working for two months and they are paid monthly, but they do not have savings. They have prepared a list of "pending issues". Which task should be prioritised?

  20. Elisa opened a bank account and has been given access to a credit card. When you receive it, they send you the secret personal identification number (PIN number) to be able to use it. What should Elisa do with the PIN number?

  21. Ms. Teresa got a loan of $ 80,000 from the bank. The annual interest rate of the loan is 15%. The monthly payments are $ 1,500. After one year, Ms. Teresa still owes $ 74,000. Another bank wants to increase its clients and offers a $ 100,000 pesos loan with an annual interest rate of 13%, but the monthly payments would also be $ 1,500. What would you advise Mrs. Teresa?

Due to the author's rights of the instrument, only the items are included (it is not possible to publish the answers and the scoring keys for each one). For more information, please write to the authors. marianela.denegri@ufrontera.cl

Received: October 05, 2018; Accepted: July 25, 2019

* Corresponding author. E-mail address:marianela.denegri@ufronteraxl

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License