SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.52Improving retention by placing retrieval practice at the end of class: a naturalistic studyEfficacy of a cognitive stimulation programme using technology on older adults’ self-esteem, self-efficacy, and autonomy author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología

Print version ISSN 0120-0534

rev.latinoam.psicol. vol.52  Bogotá June 2020  Epub Mar 01, 2021

https://doi.org/10.14349/rlp.2020.v52.4 

ARTÍCULOS

Life Satisfaction among Indigenous People from Chile: Evaluation of Measurement Invariance

Satisfacción con la vida en indígenas de Chile: evaluación de la Invarianza de Medida

1 Departamento de psicología, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile

2Centro de excelencia en psicología económica y del consumo, núcleo científico y tecnológico en ciencias sociales, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile

3Departamento de producción agropecuaria, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile

4Departamento de psicología, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco, Chile


Abstract

Although the Life Satisfaction Scale (SWLS) has been used to establish comparisons among nations/cultures, the measurement invariance of this scale has not always been demonstrated. The forced acculturative experience of indigenous minorities and its differentiation of gender roles could impact how people respond to this scale. The purpose of this study was to examine the measurement invariance of the SWLS when it is applied to a Chilean indigenous group, considering gender and their most frequent acculturative types (i.e., acculturated and bicultural). Eight hundred adults of Mapuche origin answered a Spanish version of the SWLS and the Mapuche Acculturation Scale. The results supported the SWLS strict invariance between acculturated and bicultural and also between acculturated men and women. Partial strict invariance was found between bicultural men and women; specifically, the residual variance of Item 3 was higher in women than in men. Since Spanish is a gendered language, a linguistic explanation is proposed for the non-invariance of this item. The importance of careful wording of the items, when administered in multilanguage acculturation contexts, is discussed.

Key words: Satisfaction with Life Scale; indigenous Chileans; measurement invariance; acculturation; gender

Resumen

Aunque la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida (SWLS) se ha usado para comparar personas de distintas naciones/culturas, no siempre su invarianza de medición ha sido demostrada. La aculturación forzada de las minorías indígenas y sus particulares roles de género podrían afectar las respuestas de esas personas a esta escala. Este estudio tuvo por objetivo examinar la invarianza de la SWLS en un grupo indígena chileno, considerando el género y los tipos aculturativos más frecuentes (i.e., aculturados y biculturales). Ochocientos adultos mapuches respondieron, en castellano, la SWLS y la Escala de Aculturación Mapuche. Se encontró apoyo para invarianza estricta entre aculturados y biculturales y, además, entre hombres y mujeres aculturados. Se encontró invarianza estricta parcial entre hombres y mujeres biculturales; específicamente, la varianza residual del ítem 3 fue mayor en mujeres que en hombres. Siendo el castellano un idioma con distinción de géneros, se propuso una explicación lingüística para la falta de invarianza de ese ítem. Se discute la importancia de redactar cuidadosamente los ítems cuando son administrados en contextos aculturativos multilingües.

Palabras clave: Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida; chilenos indígenas; invarianza de medición; aculturación; género

Introduction

In recent years there has been a growing interest in researching positive aspects of the human experience (e.g., Goodman, Disabato, Kashdan, & Kauffman, 2017). One of these aspects is life satisfaction, which refers to the degree to which a person favorably evaluates their own life, based on their subjective point of view (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). This definition of life satisfaction alludes to the cognitive component of subjective well-being (Pavot & Diener, 2009).

The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS, Diener, et al., 1985) is one of the most frequently employed instruments used to obtain an overall assessment of life satisfaction. The unidimensional structure of the SWLS has been widely replicated in numerous nations/cultures (e.g., Pavot & Diener, 2009), including different Chilean populations (e.g., Vera-Villarroel, Urzúa, Pavéz, Celis-Atenas, & Silva, 2012).

However, the separate validation of the scale’s structure within each different population group does not allow researchers to determine if these groups interpret the items to which they are responding to in the same way (Greenfield, 1997). To ensure the understanding of the items across groups, it is necessary to evaluate the invariance of the scale until, hopefully, its most demanding level is reached (Byrne, 2008).

Studies that have examined the invariance of SWLS across national/cultural groups (Atienza, Balaguer, Corte-Real, & Fonseca, 2016; Casas et al., 2012; Dimitrova & Dominguez Espinosa, 2015; Jovanović & Brdar, 2018; Schnettler et al., 2017; Whisman & Judd, 2016; Zanon, Bardagi, Layous, & Hultz, 2014) have seldom achieved the strictest level of invariance. Such finding suggests that cultural differences could cause respondents to have different interpretations of at least some of the items on the SWLS (Gálvez-Nieto, Salvo, Pérez-Luco, Hederich, & Trizano-Hermosilla, 2017; Oishi & Gilbert, 2016).

The cultural reality of indigenous minorities could be a potential source of non-invariance. Indigenous populations from around the world have faced genocide, colonization, forced assimilation, and exclusion, provoking lifelong trauma and loss. Such circumstances have significantly undermined their overall well-being (Brave Heart, Chase, Elkins, & Altschul, 2011).

Through their drastic interaction with mainstream society, indigenous minorities have experienced acculturative changes that, in turn, have resulted in either maintaining or not their own culture (Berry, Phinney, Sam, & Vedder, 2006; Guitart, 2010). Depending on whether ancestral culture and values are preserved and/or the adoption of the mainstream culture is imposed, members of indigenous groups may interpret and respond to questionnaire items differently, especially if the questionnaire was constructed within another cultural context (Davidov, Meuleman, Cieciuch, Schmidt, & Billiet, 2014). Thus, it is necessary to assess whether mainstream instruments are valid and culturally appropriate measures to evaluate ethnic minorities. This study is the first to examine the invariance of the SWLS among native Chileans, the Mapuches, considering their acculturative heterogeneity.

The Present Study

In Latin America there are currently 826 indigenous groups, which represent 7.8% of the population (Banco Mundial, 2015). The Mapuches are the largest Chilean indigenous group, representing 9.9% of the national population (Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas, 2018). They are concentrated in the Araucanía region (34.9%) and the Metropolitan region, which includes Santiago, the country’s capital (36.5%).

Like other indigenous minorities, the Mapuches have experienced poverty, discrimination (Programa de las Naciones Unidas para el Desarrollo, 2013) and imposed acculturative changes within the Chilean territory (e.g., Leca, 2007; Saiz, Cornejo, Fuchslocher, Holzapfel & Scheel, 1998). These acculturative changes have been marked by an asymmetric and chronic territorial conflict with the Chilean State that dates back to the late nineteenth century. This conflict forced the Mapuches into segregated rural settlements due to land usurpation. Moreover, with the military coup and the de facto government established in 1973, the Mapuches suffered severe military repression, assassination or disappearance of community members and, more critical for this study, the prohibition of their cultural practices, religion, customs and the use of their language (Minorities at Risk Project, 2009). This violent assimilation has caused a severe historical trauma for the Mapuche people (Vargas, 2017).

Previous studies (Saiz, 2003; Saiz et al., 1998) have reported that the most common acculturative groups (types) among the Mapuches are the acculturated (those with a low level of involvement in their ancestral culture and a high level of involvement in the mainstream culture) and the bicultural (those with a high level of involvement in both cultures). While both acculturative types have adopted the mainstream culture to some degree, only the bicultural conserve the ancestral culture.

Emerson, Guhn, and Gadermann (2017) reviewed three decades of research using the SWLS and found only one study that compared cultural invariance across ethnic groups (i.e., Swami & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009). Findings from the study indicated that the meaning of SWLS items not only varied across cultures, but also between genders. Based on this and previous evidence (e.g., Atienza, Balaguer, & García-Merita, 2003; Bacro et al., 2019; Bagherzadeh et al., 2018; Checa, Perales, & Espejo, 2018; Moksnes, Løhre, Byrne, & Haugan, 2014; Lorenzo-Seva et al., 2019), we will also test for gender invariance among the Mapuches, because in their traditional culture there is a noticeable gender role differentiation.

Mapuche women are usually given the role of transmitting the ancestral culture to new generations, implying that the cultural content would be more salient for women than for men (Warren, 2009). This difference in gender roles could be especially relevant among those in the bicultural group. For this reason, this study also investigated SWLS invariance between men and women within the acculturated and bicultural groups. Prior literature shows that the SWLS does not always reach the strictest level of invariance across gender in non-indigenous populations (e.g., Atienza, Balaguer, & García-Merita, 2003; Moksnes et al., 2014). It is possible that forced acculturative changes may impact the way Mapuches evaluate their life satisfaction.

This study aimed to investigate the SWLS measurement equivalence among native Chilean men and women who differ in their acculturative experiences. Specifically, the study objectives were to examine the SWLS invariance (1) between the acculturated and bicultural types, as well as (2) between men and women within the acculturated and bicultural types. It was our goal to contribute to the understanding of the role acculturation and gender may play in the psychological assessment of ethnic minorities, an essential challenge in multicultural psychology (Leong, Leung, & Cheung, 2010).

Method

Participants and Procedure

Three self-report questionnaires were given to a purposive sample of 800 adult Chileans who self-identified as Mapuches, 400 of whom were recruited in Temuco, Araucanía region, while 400 were recruited in Santiago, Metropolitan region. The resulting sample mainly consisted of people between the ages of 35 and 54 (45.3%), women (63.7%), and residents of urban zones (81.8%). The majority of participants were of a low or middle-low socioeconomic level, suggesting that most of those sampled were economically disadvantaged and, according to their age, they had experienced the consequences of the cultural repression.

All participants reported being fluent in Spanish. Among bicultural Mapuches, all were also fluent in Mapudungún, the Mapuche language. Participants responded to the questionnaires in public places known to have a high presence of Mapuches (e.g., popular markets and bus stations in the case of Temuco, and ethnically diverse communes in the case of the Metropolitan region). The pollsters read the instructions and the items of the questionnaires, recording the answers given by the participants. The study protocol was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Agricultural and Forestry Sciences School at the Universidad de La Frontera, and no material remuneration was provided in exchange for participation.

Measures

Life Satisfaction Scale (SWLS). In this study, a version of SWLS translated into Spanish by Schnettler, Miranda, Sepúlveda, and Denegri (2011) was used. In Appendix A, the translated items of the SWLS are shown along with the original English. The scale includes five items with response options ranging from “completely disagree” (1) to “completely agree” (6).

Mapuche Acculturation Scale (MAS). An abbreviated version of this scale (Saiz et at., 1998) developed by Schnettler et al. (2009) was used in this study. Based on a bidirectional acculturation model (Keefe & Padilla, 1987), MAS evaluates the respondent’s involvement in the Mapuche culture (MAS-M subscale) as well as in the mainstream Chilean culture (MAS-C subscale) according to four content areas (language, social interaction, customs, and personal and family identity). The response format provides two options: “Yes” (1) or “No” (0). Following the work of Saiz et al. (1998), the average of each participant’s responses to MAS-M (13 items) and MAS-C (7 items) was calculated. Then, these scores were divided into highs and lows using the central point of the response scale (.5) as a cut-off. The cross-tabulation of these two dichotomous variables generated four acculturative types: marginal (low involvement in both cultures), non-acculturated (high involvement in the Mapuche culture and low involvement in mainstream Chilean culture), acculturated (low involvement in the Mapuche culture and high involvement in mainstream Chilean culture) and bicultural (high involvement in both cultures). The MAS has been found to possess adequate psychometric properties in different Mapuche populations (e.g., Saiz, 2003).

Demographic survey. Each participant was asked about their self-identification as Mapuche, as well as their gender, age, region and area of residence, the educational level of the head of the household, and the family ownership of household goods.

Data analysis

Main statistical analyses were conducted in R statistical software (R Core Team, 2017). In the preliminary analyses, Mardia’s test revealed a non-normal multivariate distribution of SWLS items. After dichotomizing the MAS-M and MAS-C scores, the reliability of the cut-off points was examined using Livingston’s K2 coefficient (Gempp & Saiz, 2014). The statistical models were analysed through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012). The unifactorial model of SWLS was examined for each of the six groups of interest: acculturated, bicultural, acculturated men, acculturated women, bicultural men, and bicultural women. An adequate fit of the unifactorial baseline model in all groups was required for the subsequent evaluation of the SWLS invariance across these groups. We also evaluated the model’s reliability for the six groups using the average variance extracted (AVE) and the composite reliability (CR). An AVE greater than .50 and CR greater than .70 were indicative of appropriate construct consistency (Fornell & Larcker, 1981).

Next, multigroup confirmatory factor analyses (MG-CFA) were employed to examine SWLS invariance according to three relevant comparisons: acculturated vs. bicultural, men vs. women within the acculturated group, and men vs. women within the bicultural group. In order to accomplish this, a sequence of four progressively more restrictive models was tested following the guidelines of Byrne (2008). These analyses allowed for the evaluation of the configural(M0), metric (M1), scalar (M2), and strict (M3) models in a progressive manner (Vandenberg & Lance, 2000). These models were estimated using robust methods (Satorra & Bentler, 2011). Specifically, maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLM) was used.

Five model fit indices were calculated: the Satorra-Bentler χ2, the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) with a confidence interval of 90%. These indices were interpreted according to the goodness-of-fit criteria proposed by Marsh, Hau, and Wen (2004): CFI and TLI > .95, SRMR and RMSEA ≤ 0.08, indicate an adequate model fit.

In the sequential assessment of the models, the test of difference between models, based on the Satorra-Bentler χ2 and ∆CFI with a cut-off, point of 0.01 (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002), was used to determine if a more restrictive model showed a worse fit than the previously-examined, less restrictive model (Milfont & Fischer, 2010). If a poor fit was detected, we proceeded to evaluate the partial invariance of the most restrictive model. The partial invariance entails releasing some parameters according to the model of invariance being examined, i.e., factor loadings, intercept, and residuals for evaluating metric, scalar, and strict invariance, respectively. This was evaluated by the modification indices obtained from the Lagrange test (Sörbom, 1989).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Livingston’s K2 coefficients revealed that the dichotomization of both MAS scores was reliable: MAS-M = .99 and MAS-C = .97. When cross-tabulating these two dichotomized scores, the following distribution of acculturative types was obtained: marginal or non-acculturated (n = 15, 1.9%), acculturated (n = 331, 41.3%) and bicultural (n = 454, 56.8%). As was expected, the high frequency of respondents in the acculturated and bicultural types is similar to previously reported results (Saiz, 2003; Saiz et al., 1998). Due to their low frequency, non-acculturated and marginal participants were not considered in subsequent analyses.

Unifactorial structure of each group

The CFA revealed that the unifactorial model of the SWLS showed adequate fit (CFI and RMSEA) and satisfactory reliability (AVE and CR) in each of the six groups (see Table 1). The standardized factorial loadings, as well as their means and standard deviations, are also shown in Table 2.

Table 1 Model fit and reliability for the six groups. 

Table 2 Means (M), standard deviations (SD) and standardized factor loadings (𝛌) for SWLS items in the six groups. 

Invariance between acculturated and bicultural participants

Table 3 shows the goodness-of-fit indices for the models M0 to M3 when examining SWLS invariance between the acculturated and bicultural groups by using MG-CFA. Support was found for the configural (M0), metric (M1), scalar (M2), and strict model (M3). For all the comparisons, the S-Bχ2 model difference test and the ∆CFI showed that the most restrictive model did not show a significantly worse fit when compared to the previous one. These results indicate that the SWLS obtained the maximum level of invariance (strict invariance) between acculturated and bicultural Mapuches.

Table 3 Goodness-of-fit indices and model comparison for models testing SWLS measurement invariance between acculturated and bicultural groups (n1=454, n2=331). 

Invariance between acculturated men and women

The results shown in Table 4 revealed adequate fit levels for M0 to M3 and that the addition of successive and increasing restrictions did not result in a worse fit in each model compared to the previous model. These findings provide favourable evidence for the presence of SWLS strict invariance among acculturated men and women.

Table 4 Goodness-of-fit indices and model comparison for models testing SWLS measurement invariance among acculturated: Men vs. women (n1=104, n2=227). 

Invariance between bicultural men and women

The results in Table 5 support the existence of configural (M0), metric (M1), and scalar (M2) invariance of the SWLS between bicultural men and women. The subsequent model, strict invariance (M3), differed significantly from the previous model (M2) as revealed by the Satorra-Bentler χ2 model difference test, although the ∆CFI index was within the acceptable range proposed by Cheung and Rensvold (2002). The Lagrange test showed that the residual variance of Item 3 was not invariant between bicultural men and women. A partial strict invariance model (M3P) was then estimated, releasing the constraint associated with this item residual variance parameter. This new model showed an adequate fit and did not differ significantly from the previous model (M2). The residual variance of Item 3 in M3 among bicultural women was greater (0.476, SE = 0.065) than among bicultural men (0.299, SE = 0.050).

Table 5 Goodness-of-fit indices and model comparison for models testing SWLS measurement invariance among bicultural: Men vs. women (n1=182, n2=272). 

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the measurement invariance of SWLS across acculturative groups and gender in Mapuches, a native indigenous Chilean group. Findings provided support for the strict invariance of the SWLS among the acculturated and bicultural and also among acculturated men and women. Moreover, results supported a partial strict invariance among bicultural men and women. This partial strict invariance was explained by the residual variance of Item 3 being higher for women than for men. Such a result does not appear to be a consequence of construct bias (van de Vijver & Poortinga, 1997), but instead, a deficient wording of the item in Spanish, which included the adjective “satisfied” only in its masculine form (“satisfecho”) (see Appendix A).

Spanish is a gendered language (Prewitt-Freilino, Caswell, & Laakso, 2012). Although Spanish makes the grammatical distinction between genders, it also allows for the use of the masculine gender both exclusively (referring only to men) and inclusively (referring to both men and women in a collective sense). It is possible that the bicultural women interpreted item 3 as exclusively masculine and, thus, responded in a confusing way, thereby increasing the error variance of the item. This is also possible considering that adjectives are genderless in Mapudungún (Zúñiga, 2006) and bicultural women tend to be particularly sensitive to the gender role differentiation due to their hierarchical position in the traditional Mapuche culture (Warren, 2009).

Kroll, Dussias, Bogulski, and Kroff (2012) have acknowledged that for bilinguals, both languages are active while processing information, in this case both Spanish and Mapudungún for our bicultural participants. Moreover, the authors have stated that competing languages could cause interference, becoming a potential source of errors. There is also evidence that the testing language could deteriorate performance (e.g., Faulkner-Bond & Sireci, 2015) and/ or response consistency (Bowen, Govender, & Edwards, 2016) in multilanguage contexts. It is possible that Mapuche bicultural women, due to a higher sensibility of their own culture, could have experienced the aforementioned phenomena associated with bilingualism.

To resolve the deficient wording of Item 3, it is recommended that the response process of bicultural women to this item be examined, for instance, through cognitive interviews (Miller, Willson, Chepp, & Padilla, 2014). Two possible solutions could be rewriting the item so that it includes both genders (e.g., “Estoy satisfecho/a con mi vida”), or using a masculine version (e.g., “Estoy satisfecho con mi vida”) and a feminine version (e.g., “Estoy satisfecha con mi vida”) separately when administering the SWLS. Regardless, any modifications made to Item 3 should be empirically tested before being incorporated into the scale.

This study showed several strengths. First, this is, to our knowledge, the first research study examining the invariance of SWLS within an indigenous population, while also taking into account their acculturative heterogeneity. Second, although the Mapuches are usually considered a population that is difficult to access for researchers, a substantial sample size (n = 800) was achieved in this study, which increased the statistical power of the analyses. Third, some authors (e.g., Little, 2013) argue that it may be unreasonable to assume random error invariance across groups and, for this reason, some researchers tend to omit the examination of the strict invariance. This study addressed all levels of invariance, including the most restrictive one. Finally, in general, multi-national/cultural studies examining measurement invariance fail to propose and substantiate specific cultural explanations when a lack of invariance is found. The present study overcomes this limitation by providing a reasonable linguistic explanation for the lack of strict invariance associated with Item 3 of the SWLS. Nevertheless, this explanation still needs to be empirically tested.

Despite its strengths, there are some limitations to the current study. First, the sample cannot be considered representative as the participants were selected purposely and the study targeted only specific geographic areas. Second, all participants were adults, so it is not possible to generalize findings to other age groups. Third, the sample included more women than men and more residents of urban than rural areas. This implies that the results of the study would be more valid for those subgroups with a greater presence in the sample. Using a random sample could enable researchers to overcome the previous limitations. Finally, all instruments were administered in Spanish, even though a significant proportion of participants reported being fluent in both Spanish and Mapudungún. It would be interesting for future research to examine whether the SWLS holds its invariance in Mapudungún. Once this invariance has been demonstrated, following the recommendations of the International Test Commission (2013), future studies should offer bilingual participants the possibility of responding to the SWLS in the language they are most comfortable with.

The findings presented here provide a starting point for further examination of the measurement equivalence of SWLS among acculturally diverse indigenous peoples. We also encourage future research to explore the invariance, taking into account the age of the participants and their geographical area of residence (e.g., urban vs. rural).

In conclusion, this study demonstrated the SWLS measurement equivalence within a Chilean indigenous population, while taking into consideration acculturative types and gender differences within each type. The study also highlighted the importance of being cautious with the wording of survey items when administered in multilanguage contexts, particularly in the assessment of bicultural groups.

References

Atienza, F., Balaguer, I., Corte-Real, N., & Fonseca, A. (2016). Factorial invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in adolescents from Spain and Portugal. Psicothema, 28(3), 353-358. http://dx.doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.1Links ]

Atienza, F., Balaguer, I., & García-Merita, M. (2003). Satisfaction with Life Scale: Analysis of factorial invariance across sexes. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6), 1255-1260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00332-XLinks ]

Bacro, F., Coudronnière, C., Gaudonville, T., Galharret, J.-M., Ferrière, S., Florin, A., & Guimard, P. (2019). The French adaptation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS): Factorial structure, age, gender and time-related invariance in children and adolescents. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2019.1680359Links ]

Bagherzadeh, M., Loewe, N., Mouawad, R. G., Batista-Foguet, J. M., Araya-Castillo, L., & Thieme, C. (2018). Spanish version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale: Validation and factorial invariance analysis in Chile. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21(2), 1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2018.2Links ]

Banco Mundial. (2015). Latinoamérica índígena en el siglo XXI: Primera década (Indigenous Latinamerica in the 21st century: First decade). Washington, D.C.: Author. [ Links ]

Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., &Vedder, P. (2006). Immigrant youth: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation. Applied Psychology, 55, 303-332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00256.xLinks ]

Bowen, P., Govender, R., & Edwards, P. (2016). Validating survey measurement scales for AIDS-related knowledge and stigma among construction workers in South Africa. BMC Public Health, 16(1), 70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-2756-zLinks ]

Brave Heart, M. Y. H., Chase, J., Elkins, J., & Altschul, D. B. (2011). Historical trauma among indigenous peoples of the Americas: Concepts, research, and clinical considerations. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 43(4), 282-290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2011.628913Links ]

Byrne, B. (2008). Testing for multigroup equivalence of a measuring instrument: A walk through the process. Psicothema, 20(4), 872-882. [ Links ]

Casas, F., Sarriera, J. C., Abs, D., Coenders, G., Alfaro, J., Saforcada, E., & Tonon, G. (2012). Subjective indicators of personal well-being among adolescents. Performance and results for different scales in Latin-language speaking countries: A contribution to the international debate. Child Indicators Research, 5(1), 1-28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12187-011-9119-1Links ]

Checa, I., Perales, J., & Espejo, B. (2018). Measurement invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale by gender, age, marital status and educational level. Quality of Life Research, 28(4), 963-968. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-018-2066-2Links ]

Cheung, G., & Rensvold, R. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5Links ]

Davidov, E., Meuleman, B., Cieciuch, J., Schmidt, P., & Billiet, J. (2014). Measurement equivalence in cross-national research. Annual Review of Sociology, 40, 55-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-071913-043137Links ]

Diener, E., Emmons, R., Larsen, R., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13Links ]

Dimitrova, R., & Dominguez Espinosa, A. (2015). Measurement invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in Argentina, Mexico and Nicaragua. Social Inquiry into Well-Being, 1(1), 32-39. http://dx.doi.org/10.13165/SIIW-15-1-1-04Links ]

Emerson, S. D., Guhn, M., & Gadermann, A. M. (2017). Measurement invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale: Reviewing three decades of research. Quality of Life Research, 26(9), 2251-2264. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/s11136-017-1552-2Links ]

Faulkner-Bond, M., & Sireci, S. (2015). Validity issues in assessing linguistic minorities. International Journal of Testing, 15(2), 114-135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15305058.2014.974763Links ]

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: Algebra and statistics. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(3), 382-388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800313Links ]

Gálvez-Nieto, J. L., Salvo, S., Pérez-Luco, R., Hederich, C., & Trizano-Hermosilla, Í. (2017). Invarianza factorial del Cuestionario para Evaluar Clima Social del Centro Escolar en estudiantes chilenos y colombianos (Factorial invariance of the Questionnaire for evaluating School Social Climate in Chilean and Colombian students). Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 49(2), 119-127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rlp.2016.09.003Links ]

Gempp, R., & Saiz, J. L. (2014). El coeficiente K2 de Livingston y la fiabilidad de una decisión dicotómica en un test psicológico (Livingston K2 coefficient and the reliabilty of a dichotomous decision in a psychological test). Universitas Psychologica, 13(1), 217-226. http://dx.doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY13-1.ecklLinks ]

Greenfield, P. (1997). You can’t take it with you. American Psychologist, 52(10), 1115-1124. Retrieved from: http://experts.umich.edu/en/publications/you-cant-take-it-with-you-why-ability-assessments-dont-cross-cultLinks ]

Goodman, F., Disabato, D., Kashdan, T., & Kauffman, S. (2017). Measuring well-being: A comparison of subjective well-being and PERMA. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13(4), 1-13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1388434Links ]

Guitart, M. E. (2010). Propiedades psicométricas y estructura factorial de la Escala de Identidad Étnica Multigrupo en español (MEIM) (Psychometric properties and factorial structure of the Spanish Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM)). Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 42(3), 405-412. [ Links ]

Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas. (2018). 2da entrega resultados definitivos Censo 2017 (Second release 2017 Census final results). Santiago, Chile: Author. [ Links ]

International Test Commission (2013). ITC guidelines on test use, version 1.2. Retrieved from: http://www.intestcom.org/files/guideline_test_use.pdfLinks ]

Jovanović, V., & Brdar, I. (2018). The cross-national measurement invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale in a sample of undergraduate students. Personality and Individual Differences, 128, 7-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.010Links ]

Keefe, S., & Padilla, A. M. (1987). Chicano ethnicity. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press. [ Links ]

Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Kroff, J. R. V. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 229-262). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [ Links ]

Leca, M. C. (2007). Système éducatif et acculturation: L’exemple des Mapuches du Chili (Educational system and acculturation: The example of the Mapuche of Chile). Caravelle, 88, 281-300. [ Links ]

Leong, F., Leung, K., & Cheung, F. (2010). Integrating cross-cultural psychology research methods into ethnic minority psychology. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(4), 590-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020127Links ]

Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [ Links ]

Lorenzo-Seva, U., Calderon, C., Ferrando, P. J., del Mar Muñoz, M., Beato, C., Ghanem, I., … Jiménez-Fonseca, P. (2019). Psychometric properties and factorial analysis of invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) in cancer patients. Quality of Life Research, 28(5), 1255-1264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11136-019-02106-yLinks ]

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cut-off values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(3), 320-341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2Links ]

Milfont, T., & Fischer, R. (2010). Testing measurement invariance across groups: Applications in cross-cultural research. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 111-121. http://dx.doi.org/10.21500/20112084.857Links ]

Miller, K., Willson, S., Chepp, V., & Padilla, J. L. (2014). Cognitive interviewing methodology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [ Links ]

Minorities at Risk Project. (2009). Assessment for indigenous peoples in Chile. Retrieved from: http://www.mar.umd.edu/assessment.asp?groupId=15501Links ]

Moksnes, U. K., Løhre, A., Byrne, D. G., & Haugan, G. (2014). Satisfaction with Life Scale in adolescents: Evaluation of factor structure and gender invariance in a Norwegian sample. Social Indicators Research, 118(2), 657-671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0451-3Links ]

Oishi, S., & Gilbert, E. (2016). Current and future directions in culture and happiness research. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 54-58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.005Links ]

Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2009). Review of the Satisfaction with Life Scale. In E. Diener (Ed.), Social indicators research series: Vol. 39. Assessing well-being: The collected works of Ed Diener (p. 101-117). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2354-4_5Links ]

Prewitt-Freilino, J., Caswell, T., & Laakso, E. (2012). The gendering of language: A comparison of gender equality in countries with gendered, natural gender, and genderless languages. Sex Roles, 66(3-4), 268-281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-0083-5Links ]

Programa de las Naciones Unidas para el Desarrollo. (2013). Primera encuesta de relaciones interculturales, 2012 (First intercultural relations survey, 2012), Santiago, Chile: Author . [ Links ]

R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from: https://www.r-project.org/Links ]

Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02Links ]

Saiz, J. L. (2003). Valores en estudiantes universitarios mapuches: Una visión transcultural de su contenido, estructura, y jerarquía (Values in Mapuche university students: A cross-cultural view of their content, structure, and hierarchy) (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile. [ Links ]

Saiz, J.L., Cornejo, A., Fuchslocher, V., Holzapfel, J., & Scheel, M. (1998). La Escala de Aculturación Mapuche: Diferenciando la dirección aculturativa de indígenas chilenos (The Mapuche Acculturation Scale: Differentiating the acculturative track of Chilean natives). Revista de Psicología Social y Personalidad, 14 (1), 15-28. [ Links ]

Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. (2011). Scaling corrections for statistics in covariance structure analysis (Online document). Retrieved from: http://escholarship.org/uc/item/8dv7p2hrLinks ]

Schnettler, B., Huaiquinir, V., Mora, M., Miranda, H., Sepulveda, J., & Denegri, M. (2009). Differences in food consumption associated with ethnic group and acculturation in the Metropolitan Region of Santiago, Chile. Archivos Latinoamericanos de Nutrición, 59(4), 407-418. [ Links ]

Schnettler, B., Miranda, H., Sepúlveda, J., & Denegri, M. (2011). Satisfacción con la alimentación y la vida, un estudio exploratorio en estudiantes de la Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco-Chile (Satisfaction with food and life, an exploratory study among students in La Frontera University, Temuco-Chile). Psicologia & Sociedade, 23(2), 426-435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-71822011000200024Links ]

Schnettler, B., Miranda-Zapata, E., Lobos, G., Lapo, M. C., Adasme-Berríos, C., & Hueche, C. (2017). Measurement invariance in the Satisfaction with Life Scale in Chilean and Ecuadorian older adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 110, 96-101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.036Links ]

Sörbom, D. (1989). Model modification. Psychometrika, 54(3), 371-384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02294623Links ]

Swami, V., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Psychometric evaluation of the Malay Satisfaction with Life Scale. Social Indicators Research, 92(1), 25. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/s11205-008-9295-7Links ]

Vandenberg, R., & Lance, C. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3(1), 4-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442810031002Links ]

van de Vijver, F. J., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-cultural assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13(1), 29-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.13.1.29Links ]

Vargas, R. (2017). Pewmas/sueños de justicia. Lonkos y dirigentes mapuche versus Chile en la Corte Interamericana (Pewmas/dreams of justice: Lonkos and Mapuche leaders versus Chile in the Interamerican Court). Santiago, Chile: LOM. [ Links ]

Vera-Villarroel, P., Urzúa, A., Pavéz, P., Celis-Atenas, K., & Silva, J. (2012). Evaluation of subjective well-being: Analysis of the satisfaction with life in Chilean population. Universitas Psychologica, 11(3), 719-727. [ Links ]

Warren, S. (2009). How will we recognize each other as Mapuche? Gender and ethnic identity performances in Argentina. Gender & Society, 23(6), 768-789. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0891243209351293Links ]

Whisman, M., & Judd, C. (2016). A cross-national analysis of measurement invariance of the Satisfaction with Life Scale. Psychological Assessment, 28(2), 239-244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000181Links ]

Zanon, C., Bardagi, M. P., Layous, K., & Hutz, C. S. (2014). Validation of the Satisfaction with Life Scale to Brazilians: Evidences of measurement non-invariance across Brazil and US. Social Indicators Research , 119(1), 443-453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0478-5Links ]

Zúñiga, F. (2006). Mapudungun: El habla mapuche. (Mapudungun: The Mapuche language). Santiago, Chile: Centro de Estudios Públicos. [ Links ]

Appendix A:

English- and Spanish- language versions of the SWLS 

Received: February 05, 2019; Accepted: November 29, 2019

*Autor de correspondencia: Camila Salazar-Fernández, correo electrónico: camila.salazar@ufrontera.cl

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License