SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.54Mentalidad machista, estilos de crianza y vulnerabilidad social en cuidadores primarios. Una comparación transcultural entre Argentina y Perú¿Influye el liderazgo del entrenador sobre la resiliencia de equipo? Efecto mediador de los conflictos intragrupo y la cohesión grupal índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología

versión impresa ISSN 0120-0534

rev.latinoam.psicol. vol.54  Bogotá ene./dic. 2022  Epub 01-Mar-2023

https://doi.org/10.14349/rlp.2022.v54.23 

ARTICLES

Dispositional mindfulness is associated with less cognitive fusion and better effortful control in young people: The mediating role of repetitive negative Thinking

Mindfulness disposicional se asocia con menor fusión cognitiva y mejor control del esfuerzo en personas jóvenes: El rol mediador del pensamiento negativo repetitivo

1 Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Bogotá, Colombia

2 Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, España

3 Universidad de los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia


Abstract

Introduction:

The scientific community is taking an increasing interest in mindful ness due to its positive association with physical and psychological health indicators. However, there is still ongoing debate as to whether there could be other explanations for these effects.

Aim:

This study explores the relationships between dispositional mindfulness, cognitive fusion, effortful control and repetitive negative thinking (RNT). RNT was chosen as a potential me diating variable due to its presence in multiple psychological disorders.

Method:

A mediation analysis was performed using the bootstrap method with 5000 replications on data collected from a non-clinical sample of 415 young adults who completed a battery of online questionnai res.

Results:

Dispositional mindfulness is negatively associated with measures of RNT, effortful control and cognitive fusion. The analyses indicate that the effect that dispositional mindful ness has on cognitive fusion in young people can be partially mediated with medium effect size by RNT.

Conclusion:

RNT may play a central role as an underlying process in the link between mindfulness and different psychological aspects.

Keywords: Mindfulness; repetitive negative thinking; cognitive fusion; effortful control; mediation

Resumen

Introducción:

Mindfulness ha ganado interés dentro de la comunidad científica dada su asociación con resultados positivos sobre indicadores de salud física y psicológica. Sin embargo, el debate sigue abierto sobre cuáles son los procesos que podrían mediar estas re laciones.

Objetivo:

Explorar en una muestra de 415 jóvenes, las relaciones entre mindfulness disposicional, fusión cognitiva, control del esfuerzo y pensamiento negativo repetitivo (PNR). El PNR se propone como potencial variable mediadora debido a que se es común en múltiples trastornos psicológicos.

Método:

Los análisis de mediación realizados a través de Bootstrap ping con 5000 replicaciones, indicaron que el efecto que genera el mindfulness disposicional sobre la fusión cognitiva puede estar mediado parcialmente por el PNR en personas jóvenes con un tamaño del efecto medio.

Conclusiones:

Mindfulness disposicional se relaciona de manera negativa con las medidas de PNR, control del esfuerzo y fusión cognitiva. Por tanto, el PNR podría desempeñar un papel importante como proceso subyacente entre mindfulness y diferentes aspectos psicológicos.

Palabras clave: Mindfulness; pensamiento negativo repetitivo; fusión cognitiva; control del esfuerzo; mediación

Over recent decades, both the general public and the scientific community have taken great interest in mindful ness due to its potential as a psychological technique for a range of clinical problems (Perestelo-Perez et al., 2017). Dispositional mindfulness is an innate tendency in which individuals continuously pay attention to experiences hap pening in the present with openness and non-judgement. It has been linked to reduced reactivity and better emotional response recovery (Roemer et al., 2015), as well as lower in dicators of psychopathology, such as depressive symptoms, rumination and catastrophizing (Tomlinson, 2018). Disposi tional mindfulness has also been identified as a mediating variable between anxiety and performance in young peo ple (López-Navarro et al., 2020). Similarly, skills developed through mindfulness practice have been related to lower rates of depression and anxiety symptomatology in adults (Parmentier et al., 2019) and lower rates of anxiety (Moix et al., 2021), depression, paranoia, and sensory stress in adolescents (Díaz-González et al., 2018)

Although there is currently a significant body of evidence supporting the notion that mindfulness-based protocols are effective for the treatment of anxiety, stress (Madson et al., 2018) and depression (Bonde et al., 2022), research has continued to steadily explore how mindfulness could benefit different psychological processes and related mechanisms.

A variable of particular interest in mindfulness research is repetitive negative thinking (RNT), which is a global pro cess of intrusions comprising rumination, worry, and post-event processing that has been widely linked to the main tenance of multiple psychological disorders such as anxiety and depression (Frank & Davidson, 2014; Ruiz et al., 2021). RNT has been conceptualized as a pattern of uncontrollable, intrusive, negative, and repetitive thoughts from which an individual cannot disengage easily (Ehring & Watkins, 2008). RNT is present in a wide range of clinical problems, which suggests that diverse disorders share common features of this process with only the contents of the thought differing from one disorder to another. RNT can therefore be consid ered a transdiagnostic process (Ehring et al., 2011).

Mindfulness skills contrast with typical RNT functioning. Intentional and sustained attention on the present moment opposes a fluctuating attention on past (rumination and post-event processing) or future events (worry). Mindfulness promotes a flexible and voluntary attentional deployment toward various chosen stimuli, whereas in RNT, attention is usually rigid, sustained and problem oriented. Similarly, a mindful attitude promotes a compassionate and non-judg mental approach, which is radically different from RNT’s outlook, which generally concerns judgmental and self-crit ical thoughts (Borders, 2020).

Mindfulness skills encourage a change of perspective that allows people to observe their experiences without becoming part of them. Scholars have given this ability dif ferent names, for example “decentering or reperceiving” (Shapiro et al., 2006), detachment (Shapiro, 1992) and cog nitive defusion or self-as-context (Ruiz et al., 2021). When people have little perspective or are “attached” or “fused” to their own experiences, thoughts, and other private events such as emotions and memories, these can exert dominance over overall behaviour, limiting sensitivity to contextual contingencies and causing psychological distress (Ito et al., 2021). However, there is little evidence on how these psychological processes affect, mediate, and moder ate general mindfulness skills. For example, reperceiving or decentering often overlap with indicators of mindfulness, so it is important to clarify whether these concepts refer to the same skill or not (Carmody et al., 2009).

Various studies have reported positive relationships be tween mindfulness and optimized functioning of different executive functions. For example, mindfulness has been as sociated with greater response inhibition capacity (Gallant, 2016); greater executive control by alleviating emotional interference on cognitive functions (Huang et al., 2019); greater attention and impulse control (Bueno et al., 2015); and improvements in visuospatial processing, working mem ory and general executive functioning (Zeidan et al., 2010). Tortella-Feliu et al. (2018) compared differences between meditators and non-meditators on variables such as effort ful control (an aspect related to executive attention, which includes the ability to voluntarily manage attention, inhibit a dominant response and activate a subdominant response while experiencing an emotion) and dispositional mindful ness. Their results indicate that meditators scored higher than non-meditators on indicators of attentional and inhib itory control, as well as on other facets associated with dispositional mindfulness.

Given the heterogeneity of the mediators and mecha nisms involved in mindfulness, there is a need for further study on possible processes of change, indicating that new research on mediators for mindfulness would be valuable (Burzler et al., 2019; Monteregge et al., 2020).

MacKinnon (2007) proposes two different types of medi ation studies. The first, employed after a relation between two variables has been identified, seeks to determine how a certain effect occurs through the inclusion of a third var iable to improve understanding of the relation or to de termine whether it is spurious. This third variable can be considered mediating because it is part of the causal se quence of the observed relation. The second type of medi ation study uses theory concerning the mediation approach to design experiments that allow for the alteration or mod ification of variables hypothesized to be causally related to the dependent variables. These studies therefore tend to involve an intervention with control groups.

We decided that the first type of mediation study out lined above was more appropriate for the present study be cause, rather than testing the mechanisms associated with a change in a given intervention, it allows us to examine poten tial mediators in order to test and refine theories, including with cross-sectional or longitudinal data (MacKinnon, 2011).

There have been studies that have tried to clarify the relationship between RNT and dispositional mindfulness (Petrocchi & Ottaviani, 2016), however, to our knowledge, no studies have attempted to understand the value of RNT as a mediating variable between such dissimilar aspects as cognitive fusion and effortful control.

The present study aims to explore the associations be tween dispositional mindfulness, effortful control and cog nitive fusion by proposing repetitive negative thinking as a possible mediating variable using bootstrapping methods. We formulated the following hypotheses: (1) dispositional mindfulness will be negatively associated with RNT and cog nitive fusion, (2) dispositional mindfulness will be positively associated with effortful control, (3) RNT will be positive-ly associated with cognitive fusion and negatively associ ated with effortful control, and (4) RNT will mediate the relationship between dispositional mindfulness, effortful control, and cognitive fusion.

Method

Participants

415 people over 18 years of age were recruited through a public call for participants at a Spanish university. Of the total number of participants, 51 were male, 362 were fe male and 2 were of another gender. The majority report ed a university level of education (353). The mean age of the participants (N = 415, SD = 3.72) was 20 years, with ages ranging from 18 to 47 years old. Table 1 shows the frequen cy distribution of gender and level of education.

Table 1 Sociodemographic data 

Procedure

Between November and December 2021, a call for par ticipants was made on a communication platform of a Span ish university for an experiment about mindfulness and associated psychological variables that could be complet ed online. Participation was voluntary and unpaid. Study participants received one academic credit that could be used toward various courses. The lead author designed the assessment battery. Informed consent and questionnaires for the study were available through the Qualtrics platform for both mobile devices and computers and were presented in the same order as laid out below. The average time to complete the survey was 44.5 minutes. Device configura tion required the completion of all items so there were no missing data.

Measures

Five Facets of Mindfulness Questionnaire Short Form (FFMQ). The FFMQ (Baer et al., 2012) was used to assess dispositional mindfulness. The questionnaire consists of 24 Likert-type items in which participants are asked to respond to questions on situations in their lives using a scale from 0 (never) to 5 (always). FFMQ covers five factors: observing, describing, acting with awareness, not judging inner expe rience, and not reacting to inner experience (Baer et al., 2012). The FFMQ has shown adequate levels of reliability and there is acceptable evidence of its validity (Sosa & Bi anchi-Salguero, 2019).

Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ). The PTQ (Ehring et al., 2011) was used to assess repetitive negative thinking. The questionnaire consists of a 15-item Likert-type scale in which participants respond to statements on the frequency of repetitive negative thinking on a scale from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). It assesses the different facets of the RNT process, namely repetitiveness, intrusiveness, difficulty of stopping, unproductiveness, and the extent to which it captures or interferes with mental capacity. The reliability indices of the scores on this scale are adequate and the validity of this instrument has been proven for var ious populations (Devynck et al., 2017).

Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire (CFQ). We included the CFQ (Gillanders et al., 2014) to assess cognitive fusion. The questionnaire consists of 7 Likert-type items, using a scale from 1 (never) to 7 (always). It produces a general measure of cognitive fusion, which is a verbal cognitive pro cess in which individuals become “entangled” with their own thoughts, evaluations, memories, and emotions. It has been observed that cognitive fusion can lead to the imple mentation of experiential avoidance and suppression strat egies, what worsens emotional symptoms, mindfulness, and life satisfaction. Studies have demonstrated the good psy chometric properties of Spanish-language versions of the CFQ (Ruiz et al., 2017).

Effortful Control Scale (EC). We employed the EC (Ev ans & Rothbart, 2007) to assess effortful control. The EC is part of the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ) short form which was developed to obtain a number of clinical indicators associated with temperament. Effortful control is related to executive attention and includes the ability to voluntarily manage attention, to inhibit a dominant re sponse and to activate a subdominant response while expe riencing emotion. The Effortful Control subscale consists of 19 items to be rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (strong ly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). EC is divided into three subscales: inhibitory control (7 items), attentional control (5 items) and activation control (7 items). This instrument shows acceptable levels of internal consistency, temporal stability and convergent validity (Tortella-Feliu et al., 2013).

Data analysis

All analyses were performed using the open-access sta tistical software JASP v0.16.4 (2022). The first step was to perform normality analyses using the Shapiro-Wilk test, which showed that all variables presented a normal distri bution, except for the cognitive fusion variable. We then implemented correlation and mediation analyses to ex plore the existence of indirect effects associated with the mediating variable and the relations between them. The mediation analysis was performed using the bias-corrected percentile method (Bootstrap) suggested by Biesanz et al. (2010). This type of analysis makes it possible to make caus al interpretations even in correlational and cross-sectional designs, if these are supported theoretically and with ade quate substantiation (Hayes & Preacher, 2014).

Results

Correlation analysis

To test the first three hypotheses, parametric tests (Pearson’s) were employed to estimate correlations be tween the study variables. Statistically significant negative correlations (p < .001) were identified between mindfulness (FFMQ), repetitive negative thinking (PTQ), and cognitive fusion (CFQ), and between RNT and effortful control (EC). Positive correlations were identified between mindfulness and effortful control and between RNT and cognitive fusion (p < .001). Figure 1 shows a heat map of all the correlations to facilitate the visual analysis of these results.

Figure 1 Heat map correlations (Pearson’s) * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 

Cognitive fusion and RNT both show strong associa tions with mindfulness, and on the basis of these results we decided to apply a mediation model to test our fourth hypothesis.

Mediation analysis

Following Baron and Kenny’s (1986) model for statistical ly testing mediation, we implemented bootstrapping meth ods which apply a non-parametric approach to sensitively determine how an independent variable indirectly affects the dependent variables through a mediator. Bootstrapping methods have certain advantages in testing indirect ef fects compared to classical parametric analyses such as the causal step approach or the Sobel test (Fritz & MacKinnon, 2007). For example, they do not involve any of the assump tions required of other types of analysis and they also tend to perform better with small samples in terms of statistical power and type 1 errors. For these reasons they are one of the preferred methods for making causal inferences (Hayes & Preacher, 2010).

The indirect effect is calculated on the basis of the re sampling with replacement, and a sampling distribution is then generated empirically. For this analysis the effects were tested on the basis of the standard errors of 5000 bootstrap samples. This allowed us to estimate p-values of the model test statistics and standard errors of the param eters under non-normal data conditions (Nevitt & Hancock, 2001). To determine whether the gender of the participants affected the relations identified in the model, this variable was introduced as a possible moderator with no change in the effects between the observed variables. Figure 2 pre sents the results of the mediation model with a 95% confi dence interval.

Figure 2 Mediation model showing relations between repeti tive negative thinking (PTQ), dispositional mindfulness (FFMQ), cognitive fusion (CFQ) and effortful control (EC). 

Direct effects. Direct effects represent the impact made by the independent variable on the dependent varia ble through the presence of the mediator. All direct effects of mindfulness on the dependent variables were found to be significant: cognitive fusion (M = -.134, SE = .026, CI 95%: -.187, -.080, p < . 001) and effortful control ( M = -.021, SE = .004, CI 95%: .012, .029, p < .001).

Indirect effects. Indirect effects represent the impact made by the independent variable on the dependent var iable through the mediating variable. In this regard, dis positional mindfulness affects cognitive fusion negatively (M = -.362, SE = .028, CI 95%: -.422, -.306, p < .001) and ef fortful control positively (M = .010, SE = .002, CI 95% .004, .015, p < .001) through the mediator, RNT.

Total effects . Total effects represent the impact made by the independent variable on the dependent variable without mediator involvement. We found that dispositional mindfulness affects cognitive fusion (M = -.0497, SE = .030, CI 95%: -.554, -.434, p < .001) and effortful control (M = .031, SE = .003, CI 95%: .024, 0.037, p < .001) significantly, with confidence intervals that did not overlap with zero. Details of these results are presented in Tables 2, 3 and 4.

Table 2 Direct effects 

Delta method standard errors, bias-corrected percentile bootstrap confidence intervals, ML estimator.

Table 3 Indirect effects 

Delta method standard errors, bias-corrected percentile bootstrap confidence intervals, ML estimator.

Table 4 Total effects 

Delta method standard errors, bias-corrected percentile bootstrap confidence intervals, ML estimator.

Effect size. Following the recommendations made by Cheung (2009), the standardized indirect effect value was used to measure the effect size because it is one of the most appropriate parameters for effect-size measures in media tion analyses. This estimate can be interpreted following the conventions suggested by Cohen (1988) where values of 0, 0.14, 0.36 and 0.51 correspond to effect sizes “zero”, “small”, “medium” and “large”, respectively (Cheung, 2007). In our model, we found a medium effect size for mindfulness, RNT and cognitive fusion (-.36; CI: -.422, -.306, p < .001) and a small effect size for mindfulness, RNT and effortful control (.010; CI: .004,.015, p < .001).

Discussion

In this sample of young people, statistically significant positive relationships were found between dispositional mindfulness and effortful control and negative relationships were found between mindfulness and cognitive fusion. RNT was positively associated with cognitive fusion and nega tively associated with effortful control. These results are compatible with our first three hypotheses, which in turn are consistent with the theoretical proposals of mindful ness and acceptance-based models such as Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and Metacognitive Therapy, in which verbal or metacognitive mechanisms such as decentering or defusion are seen as central elements of healthy psycholog ical functioning (Bernstein et al., 2015; Ruiz et al., 2021).

In regard to our last hypothesis about the possible me diating role of RNT, it seems that RNT acts as a partial me diator between mindfulness, cognitive fusion, and effortful control, meaning that dispositional mindfulness might exert part of its influence through the mediator (RNT), but that it also might directly influence effortful control and cogni tive fusion. This contrasts our hypothesis on the important mediating role played by repetitive negative thought. How ever, this result is not surprising, since, as Baron and Kenny (1986) have pointed out, at least in the field of psychology, complete mediation is rare due to the prevalence of multi ple mediators. This leaves the door open for further studies to identify additional potential mediators.

This result is also consistent with research showing that mindfulness training may improve self-compassion and psychological health in young people (Joss et al., 2019) by bringing an exploratory attention to the present experi ence that facilitates opportunities to explore one’s habits of mind in more depth, and reduce attempts to control or manipulate cognitive and affective experiences (Santarnec chi et al., 2014) and decrease the cognitive interference associated with this RNT process (Harper et al., 2022). All of these findings could be of particular interest for educators and clinicians who work with related issues.

According to Parmentier et al. (2019), worry and rumi nation are two of the most potent mediating variables be tween mindfulness and symptoms of anxiety and depres sion (compared with other emotional regulation strategies such as reappraisal and suppression). Likewise, dispositional mindfulness has been negatively related to mental health problems, with this relationship being mediated by process es such as decentering and self-acceptance, which position decentering as key to understanding the mechanisms as sociated with the effects of mindfulness (Ma & Siu, 2020).

Decentering and defusion are both variables linked to mindfulness (Brown et al., 2015; Hayes et al., 2014; Vanzhu la & Levinson, 2020). These two variables could be consid ered analogous, as they differ only in the nuances of their conception and both constitute the result of a sequence of processes that are altered through mindfulness skills, whether these be dispositional or developed through ther apeutic exercises.

Assaz et al. (2018) propose that this type of mechanism could be analysed using a basic process approach in order explain the changes associated with the implementation of strategies that promote processes such as defusion. The changes are thought to occur because different mecha nisms such as responsive extinction, counterconditioning, inhibitory learning, differential reinforcement, and the introduction of verbal cues work together to reduce the influence of verbal relations on other behaviours. This ap proach, based on processes of change, represents a new paradigm in clinical psychology. It moves forward from a nomothetic approach towards the identification of common modifiable elements that currently make up different ther apeutic strategies, facilitating the integration of multiple theoretical orientations as part of an integrative proposal that uses an evidence-based practice approach (Hofmann & Hayes 2019; Sanford et al., 2022).

The limitations of this study must be taken into account when analysing its results. Even though we controlled for the gender variable in our analyses, most of the partici pants in the sample were female. This could represent a behavioural bias in the observed variables, because gender differences have been reported in several indicators asso ciated with psychological functioning and mental health (Matud et al., 2019). Likewise, the mean age did not exceed 20 years, which makes it difficult to generalize these results to other types of populations and contexts. Accordingly, fu ture research could consider strategies to ensure that the sample of participants is better balanced, and its age range expanded, in order to be able to identify possible differenc es associated with the participants’ gender and age in the indicators of dispositional mindfulness, repetitive negative thinking, effortful control and cognitive fusion.

The high correlation between repetitive negative think ing and cognitive fusion needs to be analysed. This result could reflect three different hypotheses. First, it could sim ply be related to the measurement instruments used, due to the measures’ insufficient discriminant validity for as sessing both the PTQ and the CFQ (Valencia, 2020). Second ly, the high correlation could indicate an overlap between variables, in which case they would be measuring aspects of the same construct that encompass both elements, as is the case of experiential avoidance (Mansell & McEvoy, 2017). The last possible explanation assumes that both re petitive negative thinking and cognitive fusion are part of the same pattern of inflexible behaviour, distinguishing the former as a process that involves changes in covert behav iour (thoughts) under the influence of contextual events, while cognitive fusion would be the result of this series of behavioural events. In any case, the close relationship found in this study between the two constructs does not allow any firm conclusions to be drawn on the extent of the results until the relation between these two variables is determined.

Taking into consideration that in certain cases, patients with high levels of RNT tend to be less responsive to treat ment (Lewis et al., 2021), the identification of RNT and its subtypes (rumination, worry and post-event processing) as possible mediating variables of the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and other mechanisms involved in the maintenance of psychological problems may represent possibilities for improving the effectiveness of treatments. This also reinforces the idea that RNT should be considered a transdiagnostic process and therefore an intervention tar get to be considered in psychological treatments (Watkins & Roberts, 2020).

Future research could explore whether treatments fo cused on transdiagnostic processes common to various clin ical problems, such as RNT, are comparable to protocols designed for specific diagnoses, examining the acceptability of their response and maintenance rates, and what process es of change are altered through their implementation. Al though RNT has been predominantly studied in the context of disorders such as anxiety and depression, it is necessary to consider the role it may play in other types of medical and psychological problems, as there is a growing body of evidence pointing to the presence of this process in patients with epilepsy (Whitfield et al., 2022), insomnia (Cheng et al., 2020), fatigue (Leung et al., 2022), and substance abuse (Hamonniere et al., 2022), as well as in the emo tional symptomatology associated with mothers in the perinatal period (Moulds et al., 2022)1 2 3.

References

Assaz, D.A., Roche, B., Kanter, J.W., & Oshiro, C. K. B. (2018). Cog nitive defusion in acceptance and commitment therapy: What are the basic processes of change? The Psychological Record, 68(4), 405-418. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0254-zLinks ]

Baer, R. A., Carmody, J., & Hunsinger, M. (2012). Weekly change in mindfulness and perceived stress in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(7), 755-765. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.21865Links ]

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Concep tual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Per sonality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173Links ]

Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., Lichtash, Y., Tanay, G., Shepherd, K., & Fresco, D. M. (2015). Decentering and related con structs. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 599-617. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615594577Links ]

Biesanz, J. C., Falk, C. F., & Savalei, V. (2010). Assessing mediation al models: Testing and interval estimation for indirect effects. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45(4), 661-701. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2010.498292Links ]

Bonde, E. H., Fjorback, L. O., Frydenberg, M., & Juul, L. (2022). The effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction for school teachers: a cluster-randomized controlled trial. Eu ropean Journal of Public Health, 32(2), 246-253. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckab223Links ]

Borders, A. (2020). Rumination and related constructs: causes, consequences, and treatment of thinking too much. Academic Press. [ Links ]

Brown, D. B., Bravo, A. J., Roos, C. R., & Pearson, M. R. (2015). Five facets of mindfulness and psychological health: Evaluating a psychological model of the mechanisms of mindfulness. Mindfulness, 6(5), 1021-1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671- 014-0349-4Links ]

Bueno, V. F., Kozasa, E. H., da Silva, M. A., Alves, T. M., Louzã, M. R., & Pompéia, S. (2015). Mindfulness meditation improves mood, quality of life, and attention in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. BioMed Research International, 2015, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/962857Links ]

Burzler, M. A., Voracek, M., Hos, M., & Tran, U. S. (2019). Mecha nisms of mindfulness in the general population. Mindfulness, 10(3), 469-480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0988-yLinks ]

Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Olendzki, N. (2009). An empirical study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mind fulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 613-626. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20579Links ]

Cheng, P., Kalmbach, D. A., Castelan, A. C., Murugan, N., & Drake, C. L. (2020). Depression prevention in digital cognitive behav ioral therapy for insomnia: Is rumination a mediator? Journal of Affective Disorders, 273, 434-441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jad.2020.03.184Links ]

Cheung, M. W. L. (2007). Comparison of approaches to con structing confidence intervals for mediating effects using structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(2), 227-246. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510709336745Links ]

Cheung, M. W. L. (2009). Comparison of methods for constructing confidence intervals of standardized indirect effects. Behav ior Research Methods, 41(2), 425-438. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.41.2.425Links ]

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press. [ Links ]

Devynck, F., Kornacka, M., Baeyens, C., Serra, R., Neves, J. F. D., Gaudrat, B., Delille, C., Taquet, P., Depraete, O., Tison, P., Sgard, F., Rousseau, A., & Romo, L. (2017). Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): French validation of a transdi agnostic measure of repetitive negative thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02159Links ]

Díaz-González, M. C., Pérez Dueñas, C., Sánchez-Raya, A., Moriana Elvira, J. A., & Sánchez Vázquez, V. (2018). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in adolescents with mental disorders: A ran domised clinical trial. Psicothema, 30(2), 165-170. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2017.259Links ]

Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative think ing as a transdiagnostic process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1(3), 192-205. https://doi.org/10.1680/ijct.2008.1.3.192Links ]

Ehring, T., Zetsche, U., Weidacker, K., Wahl, K., Schönfeld, S., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repeti tive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Exper imental Psychiatry, 42(2), 225-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jbtep.2010.12.003Links ]

Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 868-888. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.11.002Links ]

Frank, R. I., & Davidson, J. (2014). The transdiagnostic road map to case formulation and treatment planning: Practical guidance for clinical decision making. New Harbinger Publications. [ Links ]

Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to de tect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233-239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.xLinks ]

Gallant, S. N. (2016). Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: Breaking down the benefit. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 116-130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.005Links ]

Gillanders, D. T., Bolderston, H., Bond, F. W., Dempster, M., Flax man, P. E., Campbell, L., & Remington, B. (2014). The devel opment and initial validation of the Cognitive Fusion Question naire. Behavior Therapy, 45, 83-101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.09.001Links ]

Hamonniere, T., Milan, L., & Varescon, I. (2022). Repetitive neg ative thinking, metacognitive beliefs, and their interaction as possible predictors for problematic cannabis use. Clini cal Psychology & Psychotherapy, 29(2), 706-717. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2664Links ]

Harper, N., Delgadillo, M., Erickson, A., Boese, A., Schulte, T., & Fairchild, J. K. (2022). Mindfulness attenuates the impact of worry on late-life cognitive function. Aging & Mental Health, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2021.2017851Links ]

Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2010). Quantifying and testing indi rect effects in simple mediation models when the constituent paths are nonlinear. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45(4), 627-660. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2010.498290Links ]

Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67(3), 451-470. https://doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.12028Links ]

Hayes, S. C. , Strosahl & K. D. , Wilson K.G. (2014). Terapia de acep tación y compromiso. Proceso y práctica del cambio consciente (Mindfulness). Editorial Desclée De Brouwer. [ Links ]

Hofmann, S. G., & Hayes, S. C. (2019). The future of intervention science: Process-based therapy. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 37-50. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618772296Links ]

Huang, F. Y., Hsu, A. L., Hsu, L. M., Tsai, J. S., Huang, C. M., Chao, Y. P., Hwang, T. J., & Wu, C. W. (2019). Mindfulness improves emotion regulation and executive control on bereaved indi viduals: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 541. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00541Links ]

Ito, D., Watanabe, A., & Osawa, K. (2021). Mindful attention awa reness and cognitive defusion are indirectly associated with less PTSD-like symptoms via reduced maladaptive posttrau matic cognitions and avoidance coping. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01521-0Links ]

JASP Team. (2022). JASP (Version 0.16.4) (Computer software). https://jasp-stats.org/download/Links ]

Joss, D., Khan, A., Lazar, S. W., & Teicher, M. H. (2019). Effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on self-compassion and psychological health among young adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2373. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02373Links ]

Leung, P., Li, S. H., & Graham, B. M. (2022). The relationship be tween repetitive negative thinking, sleep disturbance, and subjective fatigue in women with Generalized Anxiety Di sorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 61, 666-679. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12356Links ]

Lewis, E. M., Heimberg, R. G., Gilroy, S. P., & Buckner, J. D. (2021). The impact of brief mindfulness training on post-event proces sing among individuals with clinically elevated social anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 52(4), 785-796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. beth.2020.10.002Links ]

López-Navarro, E., Giorgetti, D., Pérez, J. M. E., & Díaz, S. A. H. (2020). Does dispositional mindfulness mediate the relationship between anxiety and exam performance? Revista de Investiga ción en Educación, 18(3), 324-331. https://doi.org/10.35869/reined.v18i3.3271Links ]

Ma, Y., & Siu, A. F. (2020). Dispositional mindfulness and mental health in Hong Kong college students: The mediating roles of decentering and self‐acceptance. Australian Journal of Psy chology, 72(2), 156-164. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12269Links ]

MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542Links ]

MacKinnon, D. P., & Luecken, L. J. (2011). Statistical analysis for identifying mediating variables in public health dentistry in terventions. Journal of Public Health Dentistry, 71(Suppl 1), S37-S46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-7325.2011.00252.xLinks ]

Madson, L., Klug, B., Stimatze, T., Eness-Potter, K., & MacDonald, J. (2018). Effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction in a community sample over 2 years. Annals of Clinical Psychia try, 30(1), 52-60. [ Links ]

Mansell, W., & McEvoy, P. M. (2017). A test of the core process account of psychopathology in a heterogenous clinical sample of anxiety and depression: A case of the blind men and the elephant? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 46, 4-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.06.008Links ]

Matud, M. P., López-Curbelo, M., & Fortes, D. (2019). Gender and psychological well-being. International Journal of Environ mental Research and Public Health, 16(19), 3531. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16193531Links ]

Moix, J., Cladellas, M., Gayete, S., Guarch, M., Heredia, I., Parpal, G., Toledo, A., Torrent, D., & Trujillo, A. (2021). Effects of a mindfulness program for university students. Clínica y Salud, 32(1), 23-28. https://doi.org/10.5093/clysa2020a24Links ]

Monteregge, S., Tsagkalidou, A., Cuijpers, P., & Spinhoven, P. (2020). The effects of different types of treatment for anxie ty on repetitive negative thinking: A meta‐analysis. Clini cal Psychology: Science and Practice, 27(2), Article e12316. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101777Links ]

Moulds, M. L., Bisby, M. A., Black, M. J., Jones, K., Harrison, V., Hirsch, C. R., & Newby, J. M. (2022). Repetitive negative thin king in the perinatal period and its relationship with anxiety and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 311, 446-462. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.05.070Links ]

Nevitt, J., & Hancock, G. R. (2001). Performance of bootstrapping approaches to model test statistics and parameter standard error estimation in structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 353-377. [ Links ]

Parmentier, F. B. R., García-Toro, M., García-Campayo, J., Yañez, A. M., Andrés, P., & Gili, M. (2019). Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population: The me diating roles of worry, rumination, reappraisal and suppres sion. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00506Links ]

Perestelo-Perez, L., Barraca, J., Peñate, W., Rivero-Santana, A., & Alvarez-Perez, Y. (2017). Mindfulness-based interventions for the treatment of depressive rumination: Systematic re view and meta-analysis. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 17(3), 282-295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2017.07.004Links ]

Petrocchi, N., & Ottaviani, C. (2016). Mindfulness facets distincti vely predict depressive symptoms after two years: The media ting role of rumination. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 92-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.017Links ]

Qualtrics. (2022). Qualtrics (Version 11.21)(Survey software). https://www.qualtrics.comLinks ]

Roemer, L., Williston, S. K., & Rollins, L. G. (2015). Mindfulness and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 52-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.006Links ]

Ruiz, F. J., Suárez-Falcón, J. C., Riaño-Hernández, D., & Gillanders, D. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire in Colombia. Revista Latinoamericana de Psico logía, 49(1), 80-87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rlp.2016.09.006Links ]

Ruiz, F. J., Gil-Luciano, B., & Segura-Vargas, M. A. (2021). Cognitive defusion. In M. P. Twohig, M. E. Levin, & J. M. Petersen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of acceptance and commitment therapy. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197550076.013.9Links ]

Sanford, B. T., Ciarrochi, J., Hofmann, S. G., Chin, F., Gates, K. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2022). Toward empirical process-based case conceptualization: An idionomic network examination of the process-based assessment tool. Journal of Contex tual Behavioral Science, 25, 10-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2022.05.006Links ]

Santarnecchi, E., D´Arista, S., Egiziano, E., Gardi, C., Petrosino, R., Vatti, G., Reda, M., & Rossi, A. (2014). Interaction between neuroanatomical and psychological changes after mindful ness-based training. PLoS ONE, 9(10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108359Links ]

Shapiro Jr., D. H. (1992). Adverse effects of meditation: A pre liminary investigation of long-term meditators. International Journal of Psychosomatics, 39(1-4), 62-67. [ Links ]

Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373-386. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20237Links ]

Sosa, M. P. & Bianchi-Salguero, J. (2019). Propiedades psicométricas de la escala Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire FFMQ en una muestra colombiana (Master’s dissertation, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz). Digital repository. https://repositorio.konradlorenz.edu.co/handle/001/2235Links ]

Tomlinson, E. R., Yousaf, O., Vittersø, A. D., & Jones, L. (2018). Dispositional mindfulness and psychological health: A systema tic review. Mindfulness, 9(1), 23-43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0762-6Links ]

Tortella-Feliu, M., Ibáñez, I., García De La Banda, G., Balle, M., Aguayo-Siquier, B., Morillas-Romero, A., & Llabrés, J. (2013). La Escala de Control Intencional para adultos: Propiedades psicométricas de la versión catalana y sus relaciones con la regulación emocional cognitiva. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 953-964.. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.135111Links ]

Tortella-Feliu, M., Soler, J., Burns, L., Cebolla, A., Elices, M., Pas cual, J. C., López Del Hoyo, Y., Baños, R., & García-Campayo, J. (2018). Relationship between effortful control and facets of mindfulness in meditators, non-meditators and individuals with borderline personality disorder. Personality and Mental Health, 12(3), 265-278. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1420Links ]

Valencia, P. D. (2020). Initial psychometric evaluation of the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire in Peruvian undergra duates. Liberabit. Revista Peruana de Psicología, 26(2), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.2020.v26n2.05Links ]

Vanzhula, I. A., & Levinson, C. A. (2020). Mindfulness in the treat ment of eating disorders: Theoretical rationale and hypothe sized mechanisms of action. Mindfulness, 11(5), 1090-1104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01343-4Links ]

Watkins, E. R., & Roberts, H. (2020). Reflecting on rumination: Consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of ru mination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 127, 103573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573Links ]

Whitfield, A., Walsh, S., Levita, L., & Reuber, M. (2022). 143 Ca tastrophising cognitions in patients with psychogenic non-epi leptic seizures compared to patients with epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 93(6), A54.3-A54. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2022-abn.175Links ]

Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010).Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 597-605. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014Links ]

Declarations: Ethics approval: This study was performed in line with the principles of the American Psychological Association (APA) and the Dec laration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (approval code CEI 104- 2024).

Consent to participate: All persons gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

Conflict of interest: The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Received: July 26, 2022; Accepted: October 11, 2022

* Corresponding author: Juan Camilo Vargas-Nieto, email: juanvargaspsicologia@gmail.com

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License