SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.14 issue1Dysfunctional Patterns of Food Intake by Anxiety during Isolation by COVID-19 in Chile, Colombia and MexicoA Longitudinal Analysis of Social Skills and Adolescent Depression: A Multivariate Latent Growth Approach author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


International Journal of Psychological Research

Print version ISSN 2011-2084

int.j.psychol.res. vol.14 no.1 Medellín Jan./June 2021  Epub June 15, 2021

https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.4765 

Research articles

Mood and Affective Balance of Spaniards Confined by COVID-19: A Cross-Sectional Study

Estado de ánimo y equilibrio afectivo de los españoles confinados por COVID-19: un estudio transversal

Maria del Carmen Perez-Fuentes1  * 

Maria del Mar Molero Jurado1 

Africa Martos Martinez1 

Maria del Mar Simon Marquez1 

Jose Jesus Gazquez Linares1  2 

1Facultad de Educación, Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Almería. Spain.

2Universidad Autónoma de Chile. Chile.


Abstract.

The aim of this study was to analyze the mood and affective balance of Spaniards in quarantine and determine the predictive role of sociodemographic variables and mood on the negative affective balance. This cross-sectional study was carried out with a sample of 1014 Spanish adults, 67.2% were women and 32.8% men. The age ranged from 17 to 76. The instruments used were the Mood Assessment Scale and the Affective Balance Scale, which were implemented as a CAWI survey (Computer Aided Web Interviewing). Results showed that age correlated negatively with Sadness-Depression, Anxiety, and Happiness. Women had more Sadness-Depression, Anxiety, and negative affect, while men showed more Happiness and higher positive affect. Thus, the risk of a negative affective balance during confinement was greater for women and those who showed an emotional state marked by sadness-depression and anxiety, while older age and higher scores in happiness were associated with lower risk. In conclusion, knowing which groups are at risk of emotional and affective alteration can facilitate the detection and prevention of later disorders, such as severe stress and posttraumatic stress disorder, avoiding their generalized presence, and becoming a new public health problem derived from COVID-19.

Keywords: Mood; Emotions; Mental Health; Covid-19; Spain

Resumen.

El objetivo de este trabajo fue analizar el estado de ánimo y el balance afectivo de los españoles en cuarentena y determinar el papel predictivo de las variables sociodemográficas y de los estados anímicos, sobre el balance afectivo negativo. Se trata de un estudio transversal, con una muestra de 1014 adultos españoles, siendo el 67.2% mujeres y el 32.8% restante, hombres. El rango de edad estuvo entre 18 y 76 años. Los instrumentos empleados fueron la Escala de Evaluación del Estado de Ánimo y la Escala de Equilibrio Afectivo, empleando para su implementación una encuesta CAWI (Computer Aided Web Interviewing). Los resultados mostraron que la edad correlaciona negativamente con la Tristeza-Depresión, la Ansiedad y Alegría. Las mujeres presentan mayor nivel de Tristeza-Depresión, Ansiedad y afecto negativo, mientras que los hombres presentan más Alegría y mayor nivel de afecto positivo. Por lo tanto, el riesgo de presentar un balance afectivo negativo durante el confinamiento es mayor entre mujeres y entre quienes manifiestan un estado emocional marcado por la tristeza-depresión y la ansiedad, mientras que tener mayor edad y mostrar puntuaciones elevadas en alegría se asocia con menor riesgo. En conclusión, conocer los grupos de riesgo en alteraciones emocionales y afectivas puede facilitar la detección y prevención de trastornos posteriores, como estrés agudo y trastorno de estrés postraumático, evitando la presencia generalizada, y que esto se convierta en un nuevo problema de Salud Pública derivado del COVID-19.

Palabras Clave: Estado de ánimo; emociones; salud mental; Covid-19; España.

1. Introduction

COVID-19 is the name given to the disease caused by the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARSCoV-2) (Gorbalenya et al., 2020). The worldwide outbreak of this novel coronavirus (COVID-19), which started in China, has led to a public health emergency of international concern. In absence of vaccinations and antivirals to fight it, and taking as a reference the measures taken in previous outbreaks of Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome (SARS) (Hsieh et al., 2007), quarantine and isolation are considered the most effective measures for its containment (Zhai et al., 2020).

This new coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) has rapidly propagated across countries, causing severe disease and sustained person-to-person transmission, leading to explosive outbreaks, even in confined settings (G. Li et al., 2020; Mizumoto et al., 2020). Thus, quarantine alone is not enough to prevent the virus from spreading, and it is a reason for strong global concern (Sohrabi et al., 2020). Due to the impossibility of controlling the disease and the severity of the risk, it is not surprising for the population to feel anxious about possible contagion (Zandifar & Badrfam, 2020). Worry is repetitive negative thinking about the uncertainty of future events developing under stress (Skodzik et al., 2016), in which one feels unable to cope with the threat (Soriano et al., 2019). This negative affect usually varies widely, beyond emotions like the nervousness, unease or fear typically associated with it, such as irritability and agitation (emotions quite similar to those in generalized anxiety disorder), despair, annoyance, sadness (which are expected to occur in disorders such as depression), impatience, jealousy, stress, and displeasure (Gould et al., 2018). High levels of perceived threat from COVID-19 particularly influence the appearance of a negative mood state and feelings of irritation, anxiety, despondency or sadness in the general population, which, in turn, promote a cognitive state of worry about the virus (Pérez-Fuentes et al., 2020). This state of overestimation of threat and intolerance to uncertainty, associated with symptoms of hypochondriasis and depression, may partly originate from dysfunctional beliefs about disease acquired early in life, and triggered by a critical incident (Arnáez et al., 2019).

In the evolution of worry, associated negative feelings tend to diminish with age (Arnáez et al., 2019; Pietrzak et al., 2012). However, older people are more nervous when they worry about their health (Wuthrich et al., 2015). Studies such as the one by Ro et al. (2017) have shown that worry about a pandemic affects women, those with higher daily stress, and those who think they have poor health, that is, those who perceive themselves to be more susceptible. Nevertheless, in the case of COVID-19, the epidemiological data show that the individuals at highest risk of mortality are men, more than women, and the elderly (Borges do Nascimiento et al., 2020; Shim et al., 2020).

Nevertheless, such feelings and negative emotions may even appear in those who are not at risk of becoming ill in public health events, such as the current coronavirus outbreak (Montemurro, 2020). Emotions and feelings related to worry about the disease combine with distress generated by the home confinement decreed by the state of emergency in Spain (Real Decreto 463, 2020, march 14). Zhou (2020) suggested that in countries such as China, coupled with the increase in confirmed cases and deaths, negative emotions spread through the population in quarantine, generating considerable feelings of fear, anxiety, sadness, guilt, and nervousness, and threatening the mental health of a large number of people. Quarantine, one of the oldest ways in history of containing infectious diseases, is defined as the restriction or segregation of individuals who may have come into real or potential contact with transmissible pathologies, until the time in which is certain they no longer constitute a health risk (Conti, 2017). The psychological impact of home confinement can generate posttraumatic stress symptoms, confusion, and rage. Individuals who spend most of their time in quarantine perceive an especially strong threat of infection, frustration and boredom, feel lack of supplies and adequate information, and suffer social stigmatization or financial loss (Brooks et al., 2020). Along this line, Li et al. 2020 found in a study of emotional indictors in a group, before and after the COVID-19 emergency was declared, that negative emotions (such as anxiety, depression, and hostility) and sensitivity to risk increased significantly, while positive emotions, such as happiness, decreased.

Age is another of the individual characteristics related to a stronger emotional impact of confinement. For example, in children and adolescents, uncertainty, changes in routine, and emotional changes undergone by the adults they live with can cause restlessness, distress, anxiety, sadness or worry, which are manifested through both direct externalization of these emotions and defiant behavior (Dalton et al., 2019; Ruiz-Hernández et al., 2020). Along the same line, Jiao et al. (2020) showed that isolation due to COVID-19 caused a strong emotional impact on children and adolescents, which turned into irritability, restlessness, and lack of attention, fear and nightmares. However, other authors have shown that older adults may experience more anxiety and depressive symptoms from this epidemic (Lima et al., 2020; Nguyen et al., 2020), because they are usually not as good at managing stressful events (Molero et al., 2018). These results are contrary to those found during previous outbreaks of coronavirus, where psychiatric morbidity was associated with being younger (Sim et al., 2010).

Along with age, sex is also a sociodemographic variable that has been related to the emotional impact of emergencies (Acosta & Herrera, 2018; Pulopulos et al., 2018). Among minors, girls show higher risk of developing posttraumatic stress syndrome (PTSS) after severe situations (Danese et al., 2020, Li et al. 2020. Liu et al. (2020) also showed that after evaluating posttraumatic stress symptoms in the Chinese population in quarantine because of COVID-19, women showed higher PTSS with re-experiencing negative alterations in cognition and mood, as well as hyperexcitation. Wang et al. (2020) found that the psychological impact of the COVID-19 outbreak in that country was significantly lower in men, who, however, scored higher in stress, anxiety, and depression.

Similarly, education has been related to emotional alteration during confinement because of the virus, where those with university degrees are the most affected (Liu et al., 2020; Nguyen et al., 2020). In this respect, studies done in other countries where the epidemic began before Spain showed a 25% prevalence of anxiety-related feelings among university students during confinement from COVID-19 (Cao et al., 2020). However, other studies have found that individuals with no education, compared to those who had some education, underwent the most emotional alteration, such as sadness and depression (Wang et al., 2020). This may be due to a stronger perception of risk from the virus among those with a lower education (Kim & Kim, 2018), especially considering that health education has been shown to exert a protective effect against depression, improving quality of life during confinement for the epidemic (Nguyen et al., 2020). In this line, a recent publication showed that anxiety levels in medical students during the pandemic remained stable, possibly due to knowledge of the transmission, prognosis, and prevention of the disease (Lasheras et al., 2020). Marital status is another of the sociodemographic factors which has shown a significant relationship with developing psychological alterations, such as anxiety and depression, during coronavirus outbreaks, as married couples showed fewer of these symptoms (Sim et al., 2010). Nevertheless, some studies have found that marital status and being a parent are not associated with the emotional state of people in quarantine (Nguyen et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020).

Studies on the psychological impact of previous outbreaks of SARS from coronavirus in the general population have found that around 25% develop psychiatric morbidity such as anxiety, somatic symptoms, social dysfunction, and depression, which in turn is related to later development of posttraumatic stress (Sim et al., 2010). And likewise, a recent meta-analysis showed that the proportion of depression in the general population is seven times higher since the start of the pandemic, and as high as 25% of the world population (Bueno-Notivol et al., 2020). Therefore, and as emphasized by the health consultants of some governments, any approach to the COVID-19 public health emergency must attempt to minimize the negative psychological as well as physical impacts of this virus (Horton, 2020); otherwise, if the immediate and long-term psychological effects of this global situation are ignored, the impact could be immeasurable (Dalton et al., 2019). Therefore, the objective of this study was to analyze the mood and affective balance of Spaniards in quarantine. It further attempted to determine the association of sociodemographic variables and mood with the negative affective balance. The first hypothesis posed was that sociodemographic characteristics such as age and education would be negatively associated with negative affective state and emotions of sadness, anxiety, and hostility (H1). The second hypothesis was that women and single people would have a more negative affective and emotional state than either man or couples, who would have more positive affect and emotions (H2). Finally, it was expected that with regard to the sociodemographic and emotional characteristics of the participants, women who had more negative emotions would have a higher probability of developing negative affect than older individuals with a higher education, who would be more cheerful and less likely to develop them (H3).

2. Method

2.1 Participants

The original size of the sample of the general Spanish population was N = 1043. Based on the answers to control questions (CQ) inserted in the questionnaires, cases in which random or incongruent answers were detected were discarded (-29), leaving a final sample of 1014. At the time of data collection, the participants were under home confinement due to the state of emergency decreed by the government of Spain in response to the current COVID-19 pandemic.

2.2 Instruments

An ad hoc questionnaire was used to collect sociodemographic data. Items included sex, age, marital status, care of minor children, and education.

The emotional situation at the time of evaluation was found using the Spanish Scale for Mood Assessment (Escala de Valoración del Estado de Ánimo (EVEA); Sanz, 2001). This instrument evaluates transitory moods and classifies them in four subscales (anxiety, sadnessdepression, anger-hostility, and happiness), with 16 items rating how much the participant identifies with the different moods enumerated on a scale of 0 (not at all) to 10 (very much). Reliability found was ω = 0.88, GLB=0.89 for anxiety, ω =0.88, GLB=0.90 for sadnessdepression, ω =0.96, GLB=0.97 for anger-hostility, and ω =0.85, GLB=0.88 for happiness.

Table 1 

Sadness-depression Anxiety Anger-Hostility Happiness Positive affect Negative affect
Age Pearson’s r -.130*** -.113 *** -.044 -.117*** -.010 -.213***
Upper 95% CI -.069 -.052 .017 -.056 .052 -.153
Lower 95% CI -.190 -.173 -.106 -.178 -.072 -.271
Education Pearson’s r -.062* -.024 -.114*** .022 -.081** -.075*
Upper 95% CI -.001 .037 -.052 .084 -.019 -.013
Lower 95% CI -.123 -.086 -.174 -.039 -.142 -.136

Figure 1 Mood by Sex and Minor Children 

Emotional state manifested during the past week was evaluated using the Spanish adaptation of the Affective Balance Scale (Godoy-Izquierdo et al., 2008; Warr et al., 1983). This instrument consists of 18 items answered on a Likert-type scale with three answer choices on the frequency the participant experienced the moods described in the past week (1= “hardly ever or never”, 2= “sometimes”, 3= “Often or usually”). The scale measures directly positive and negative experiences. The reliability indices found were ω =0.82, GLB=0.83 and ω =0.79, GLB=0.82, respectively.

2.3 Procedure

Data for this cross-sectional study were collected using a CAWI survey (Computer Aided Web Interviewing), which included, in addition to the validated questionnaires, a series of questions for collecting sociodemographic data. The survey was spread through the social networks (snowball sampling), and remained available for one week (specifically, the first week of confinement of the Spanish population from March 18-23, 2020). Participation was voluntary, and on the first page before answering the questionnaire, participants were provided with information on the study and its purpose, where they also had to check a box indicating they are aware of the informed consent before starting to complete the survey. Participants were also asked to answer sincerely and guaranteed the anonymity of their answers. Control questions were included throughout the questionnaire for detection of random answers. This study was approved by the University of Almeria Bioethics Committee.

2.4 Data analyses

First, frequency and descriptive analyses of sociodemographic data were performed. The association of variables was explored with the Pearson bivariate coefficient. Any differences between the groups were examined using the Student’s t-test for independent samples or ANOVA, depending on the case. The Cohens d (1988) was used as a measure of the effect size following the criteria: d < 0.50 small effect size, d from 0.50 to 0.80 medium, and d 0.80 large.

Then a binary logistic regression model was estimated using the enter method. The dependent variable for this was the Affective Balance Index, previously dichotomized and recoded as a dummy variable (0=Positive affective balance, 1=Negative affective balance). The sociodemographic variables and moods were included as predictors.

The McDonalds omega coefficient (McDonald, 1999) was estimated following the guidelines of Ventura-León and Caycho (2017) to examine the reliability of the instruments used to collect the data. The Greatest Lower Bound (GLB) was also estimated. The SPSS v. 23.0 statistical package was used for data processing and analysis.

3. Results

3.1 Sociodemographic Characteristics of the participants

The mean sample age was 40.87 (SD = 12.42) in a range of 18-76. Of these, 67.2% (n = 681) were women and 32.8% (n = 333) were men, with a mean age of 39.88 (SD = 12.35) and 42.92 (SD = 12.33), respectively.

Marital status distribution in the sample was 60.1% (n = 609) married or stable partner, 30.9% (n = 313) single, 8.1% (n = 82) divorced or separated, and 1% (n = 10) widowed. Of these, 35.9% (n = 364) had minor children in their care. Finally, with respect to education, 78.7% (n = 798) had a higher education, followed by 16% (n = 162) high school, 5% (n = 51) primary, and 0.3% (n = 3) with no education.

3.2 Mood and affective balance under home confinement

Table 1 shows the correlation matrix of each of the moods by participant age and education. Age correlated negatively with Sadness-Depression (r = .13, p <.001), Anxiety (r = .11, p < .001), and Happiness (r .11, p < .001). Level of education was negatively correlated with Sadness-Depression (r = .06, p < .05) and Anger-Hostility (r = .11, p < .001). In addition, there is a negative correlation between age and negative affect (r = .21, p < .001). Level of education was negatively correlated with both positive (r = .08, p < .01) and negative (r = .07, p < .05) affect.

A comparison of means was done for each mood by sex and by whether there were minor children in their care. Table 2 and Figure 1 show the statistically significant differences between sex in Sadness-Depression (t = 5.47, p < .001, d = .36) and Anxiety (t = 6.42, p < .001, d = .43), in which women had the highest scores in both cases. However, men scored significantly higher in Happiness (t = 2.38, p < .05, d = .15) than women. In addition, Table 2 shows the significant differences in two opposite moods: Sadness-Depression (t = 2.13, p < .05, d = .14), with a higher score in the group without children, and Happiness (t = 2.42, p < .05, d = .15), with higher scores for those with children in their care. In all cases, the effect size revealed by the Cohens d was small.

Finally, no statistically significant differences by marital status were found in any of the moods analyzed: Sadness-Depression (F = .98, p = .40), Anxiety (F = .08, p = .97), Anger-Hostility (F = .26, p = .85) or Happiness (F = .96, p = .41).

As shown in Table 2 and Figure 2, men scored significantly higher in positive affect (t = 3.98, p < .001, d = .26) than women, and women scored higher in negative affect (t = 7.40, p < .001, d = .49) than men. By care of minor children or not, differences were observed in positive affect (t = 2.03, p < .05, d = .13), where those with minor children in their care had the highest mean scores.

Table 2 Mood and Affective balance. Descriptive and Students t, by sex and minor children 

Mood Male Female t Mean Dif SE Dif 95% IC Cohen’s d
N M SD N M SD Lower Upper
Sadness-Depression 333 4.44 2.02 681 5.25 2.29 -5.47*** -.81 .14 -1.10 -.51 -.36
Anxiety 333 5.16 2.11 681 6.10 2.22 -6.42*** -.93 .14 -1.22 -.65 -.43
Anger-Hostility 333 4.71 2.54 681 4.85 2.66 -.80 -.14 .17 -.48 .20 -.05
Happiness 333 4.61 1.84 681 4.33 1.75 2.38* .28 .11 .05 .51 .15
Mood No minor children Minor children t Mean Dif SE Dif 95% IC Cohen’s d
N M SD N M SD Lower Upper
Sadness-Depression 650 5.10 2.27 364 4.78 2.16 2.13* .31 .14 .02 .60 .14
Anxiety 650 5.72 2.27 364 5.92 2.14 -1.31 -.19 .14 -.47 .09 -.08
Anger-Hostility 650 4.69 2.67 364 5.02 2.53 -1.93 -.33 .17 -.66 .00 -.12
Happiness 650 4.32 1.81 364 4.60 1.74 -2.42* -.28 0.11 -.51 -.05 -.15
Affective balance Male Female t Mean Dif SE Dif 95% IC Cohen’s d
N M SD N M SD Lower Upper
Positive Affect 333 16.97 4.05 681 15.99 3.49 3.98*** .98 .24 .05 .51 .26
Negative Affect 333 14.96 3.51 681 16.75 3.66 -7.40*** -1.78 .24 -2.26 -1.31 -.49
Affective balance No minor children Minor children t Mean Dif SE Dif 95% IC Cohen’s d
N M SD N M SD Lower Upper
Positive Affect 650 16.13 3.66 364 16.63 3.78 -2.03* -.49 .24 -.97 -.01 -.13
Negative Affect 650 16.31 3.69 364 15.90 3.73 1.66 .40 .24 -.07 .88 .10

Figure 2 Affective Balance by Sex and Minor Children 

Finally, in the comparison of mean scores on positive and negative affect by marital status, no differences were found for positive affect (F = 1.63, p = .18). However, there were statistically significant differences for negative affect (F = 4.60, p < .01, η 2 = .01) between the single (M = 16.74, SD = 3.72) and married (M = 15.82, SD = .58) groups. The following figure (Figure 3) shows mean scores on negative affect for each marital status group.

Figure 3 Negative affect. ANOVA(post hoc), by marital status 

3.3 Binary logistic regression model for predicting the negative affective balance

The sociodemographic variables and moods were taken as the predictors for estimating the logistic regression model. Polytomous variables were dichotomized by creating a dummy variable of 0 or 1. Thus, marital status was recoded as 0= partner and 1= no partner (this category includes single, divorced and widowed). The dependent variable was the affective balance index in a range of -18 to 18 points It was considered a negative affective balance if scores were below 0 and positive if they were above it, and this variable was then recoded as: 0= Positive affective balance and 1= Negative affective balance.

The results of the logistic regression analysis are presented in Table 3. The multivariate association measures found show that the likelihood of having a negative affective balance during the week of home confinement was 1.62 times higher for women than for men, 1.53 times higher with a Sadness-Depression mood, and 1.46 times higher with an Anxiety mood. On the contrary, older age and Happiness were protective variables against a negative affective balance.

The overall fit of the model (χ 2 = 540.80; df = 9; p < .001) was confirmed by the Hosmer-Lemeshow test (χ 2 = 5.94; df = 8; p = .654). The Nagelkerke R 2 indicated that 58% of the variability in the response variable could be explained by the logistic regression model. Based on case classification, probability of the logistic function being correct was also estimated at 80.8% with a false positive rate of .18 and false negative of .79.

4. Discussion

Confinement due to the COVID-19 pandemic may have repercussions on the psychological wellbeing of the general population, especially in those most vulnerable (Dalton et al., 2019; Shim et al., 2020). The objective of this study was therefore to find out the influence of certain sociodemographic variables on the emotional and affective balance. Based on the results, and according to our first hypothesis, we can state that age was associated with negative affect, sadness-depression, anxiety, and happiness. Specifically, older age was associated with a decrease in negative feelings of sadnessdepression, and anxiety, as well as negative affective state and increased happiness. This result is in agreement with those found during previous outbreaks of coronavirus, in which the younger population showed more negative emotions (Sim et al., 2010). It was also shown that a higher education is related with fewer negative feelings of sadness and anger-hostility, as in other studies done during the epidemic in other countries (Liu et al., 2020; Nguyen et al., 2020). Concerning the affective situation, a higher education was associated with both low positive and negative affective states. Thus, it seems that higher education enables adequate comprehension of information on COVID-19, and it would help reduce emotional alterations that arise when perceived risk is high, without this impeding understanding the real implications of the virus, and therefore, altering the positive affective state (Kim & Kim, 2018; Lasheras et al., 2020).

Turning attention to our second hypothesis, no significant differences in emotions were found by marital status; however, negative affect was higher among married couples than singles, while caring minor children was related to higher levels of happiness, and not having care of children with higher scores in sadness-depression. This differs from the results of studies done in a Chinese population, where no relationship was found between these family variables and the emotional state of those in quarantine (Nguyen et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020).

Furthermore, in the relationship of sex to emotional state, men were found to show more happiness and a positive affective state, while women showed a higher score in negative affective state and more feelings of sadnessdepression and anxiety, in line with other studies (Wang et al., 2020). Although this study evaluated emotional states at the time of assessment and affective balance in the past week of confinement, these results support the relationship shown in other studies between women and emotional alterations after quarantine, such as the appearance of posttraumatic stress symptoms (Liu et al., 2020).

Finally, to test the third hypothesis, the sociodemographic variables were analyzed to find out whether they were associated with a higher probability of developing a negative affective state during quarantine. In fact, being a woman and scoring high on negative feelings, in particular sadness-depression and anxiety, were associated with a greater possibility of showing a negative affective balance, while older age and happiness were variables related to lower risk. This model, which explained much of the variability in the negative affective balance in the sample, was in harmony with previous literature. A study by Liu et al. (2020) showed that being a woman was a risk variable for developing negative emotional states. Thus, women perceived more concern for the disease, which had a stronger impact on their affective level during the pandemic (Ro et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2020), while emotional symptoms, such as anxiety or depression, are related to later development of affective alteration (Sim et al., 2010). The result, showing age as a variable associated with lower risk, should be taken with precaution. The mean age of the sample was not very high, and very few individuals were within the age group where there is high risk of mortality from COVID-19 (Borges do Nascimiento et al., 2020; Shim et al., 2020); therefore, negative emotional states linked to higher perceived risk may not have been present (Gould et al., 2018; Zandifar & Badrfam, 2020). Thus, young age has been linked to higher psychiatric morbidity in outbreaks of epidemics (Sim et al., 2010).

Table 3 Results derived from the logistic regression for the probability of a negative affective balance 

Variables β p-value OR 95% CI
Age -.016 .045 .984 .969, 1.000
Sex (a) .484 .013 1.622 1.109, 2.374
Marital status (b) .236 .252 1.266 .846, 1.895
Minor children (c) -.240 .247 .787 .524, 1.181
Education .065 .702 1.067 .765, 1.489
Sadness-Depression .429 .000 1.536 1.366, 1.727
Anxiety .381 .000 1.464 1.294, 1.657
Anger-Hostility .006 .899 1.006 .922, 1.097
Happiness -.659 .000 .517 .455, .588
Constant -1.460 .068 .232

4.1 Limitations of this study

Some limitations should be pointed out. In the first place, the presence of COVID-19 symptoms or diagnosis was not considered, in either the participant or in close friends and family, or perceived health condition, which could affect the emotional and affective impact during confinement of the general population, and this detracts from the possibility of comparing our results. Moreover, as it has already briefly been mentioned above, the mean age of the sample was around 40; therefore, we cannot be sure whether age was a constant protective variable or only linked to adulthood. Furthermore, the study design was cross-sectional, and data were collected during the first week of confinement; consequently, no conclusions related to the evolution of the variables analyzed are possible. A longitudinal study would enable progress in the analysis of the variables included in this study. Similarly, future studies should include other variables, such as participants place of residence or economic and employment status, which could have an essential role in the cognitive and emotional result of the pandemic. Perceived threat and economic situation have been demonstrated to influence emotions felt toward COVID-19 (Brooks et al., 2020), and the resident population in the areas of healthcare most affected by the virus or where the economic situation is under duress due to the pandemic could experience more negative emotions.

5. Conclusions

The coronavirus is generating a scenario of uncertainty in which primary importance is now given to impeding increase in the rate of infection and the death toll. However, as shown by studies done in populations where the outbreak began before it arrived in Spain, expansion of negative emotions during confinement is almost as rapid as the virus itself. The psychological impact of confinement may be severe; therefore, it is indispensable to give public attention to psychological problems derived from confinement, and measures should be prepared to prevent future emotional disorders, such as severe stress and posttraumatic stress syndrome. To date, there are few studies analyzing the role of sociodemographic variables in the emotional and affective impact on the general population during states of confinement, and those that have analyzed them for COVID-19 are even fewer. In the light of the findings of this study, it seems that special attention should focus on young women experiencing emotions such as sadness or anxiety during confinement, and single people, those without children or with a low level of education, as these characteristics were related to a higher probability of showing a negative affective state. On the contrary, maturity and feelings of happiness are linked to a lower pres ence of negative affect. Thus, knowing the groups with greater likelihood of developing emotional and affective alterations can facilitate the detection and prevention of later disorders, which could become an important public health problem after the COVID-19 pandemic.

7. Contributors Statement

M.d.C.P.F., M.d.M.M.J., M.d.M.S.M. and A.M.M. contributed to the conception and design of the review. J.J.G.L. applied the search strategy. All authors applied the selection criteria. All authors completed the assessment of bias risk. All authors analyzed and interpreted the data. M.d.M.M.J., M.d.C.P.F., and A.M.M. wrote this manuscript. M.d.C.P.F. and J.J.G.L. edited this manuscript. M.d.M.M.J. is responsible for the overall project.

8. Funding

This research received no external funding.

9. Ethics Approval

The Bioethics Committee of the University of Almeria approved the study.

10. Data Sharing Statement

Data may be obtained on request from a third party but are not publicly available.

6. Acknowledgments

The present study was undertaken in collaboration with the Almeria Provincial Government. Part of the work received funding from Gerty Cori Aid for University Teaching Training in Deficit Areas granted María del Mar Simón Márquez for assistance in hiring predoctoral research staff.

References

Acosta, F., & Herrera, F. (2018). Anxiety and happiness as opposite emotional states in childhood. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 8 (2), 65-77. https://doi.org/10.30552/ejihpe.v8i2.245. [ Links ]

Arnáez, S., García-Soriano, G., López-Santiago, J., & Belloch, A. (2019). The spanish validation of the short health anxiety inventory: Psychometric properties and clinical utility. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 19 (3), 251-260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2019.05.003. [ Links ]

Borges do Nascimiento, I. J., acic, N., Abdulazeem, H. M., von Groote, T. C., Jayarajah, U., Weerasekara, I., Esfahani, M. A., Civile, V. T., Marusic, A., Jeroncic, A., Carvas Junior, N., Pericic, T. P., Zakarija-Grkovic, I., Meirelles Guimarães, S. M., Luigi Bragazzi, N., Bjorklund, M., Sofi-Mahmudi, A., Altujjar, M., Tian, M., . . . Marcolino, M. S. (2020). Novel coronavirus infection (covid-19) in humans: A scoping review and meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9 (4), Article 941. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9040941. [ Links ]

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., & Rubin, G. J. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet, 395 (10227), 912-920. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8. [ Links ]

Bueno-Notivol, J., Gracia-García, P., Olaya, B., Lasheras, I., López-Antón, R., & Santabárbara, J. (2020). Prevalence of depression during the covid-19 outbreak: A meta-analysis of communitybased studies. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 21 (1), Article 100196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2020.07.007. [ Links ]

Cao, W., Fang, Z., Huo, G., Han, M., Xu, X., Dong, J., & Zheng, J. (2020). The psychological impact of the covid-19 epidemic on college students in china. Psychiatry Research, 287, Article 112934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112934. [ Links ]

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd Ed.) Erlbaum. [ Links ]

Conti, A. A. (2017). Quarantine through History. In S. R. Quah (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Public Health (2nd ed.) Elsevier. [ Links ]

Dalton, L., Rapa, E., & Stein, A. (2019). Protecting the psychological health of children through effective communication about COVID-19. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 4 (5), 346-347. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30097-3. [ Links ]

Danese, A., Smith, P., Chitsabesan, P., & Dubicka, B. (2020). Child and adolescent mental health amidst emergencies and disasters. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 216 (3), 159-162. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.244. [ Links ]

Godoy-Izquierdo, D., Martínez, A., & Godoy, J. F. (2008). La ńEscala de Balance Afectivoż. Propiedades psicométricas de un instrumento para la medida del afecto positivo y negativo en población española [The ńAffective Balance Scależ. Psychometric properties of an instrument to measure positive and negative affect in the Spanish population]. Clínica y Salud, 19 (2), 175-189. [ Links ]

Gorbalenya, A. E., Baker, S. C., Barix, R. S., de Groot, R. J., Drosten, C., Gulyaeva, A. A., Haagmans, B. L., Lauber, C., Leontovich, A. M., Neuman, B. W., Penzar, D., Perlman, S., Poon, L. L. M., Samborskiy, D. V., Sidorov, I. A., Sola, I., & Ziebuhr, J. (2020). The species severe acute respiratory syndromerelated coronavirus: Classifying 2019-ncov and naming it sars-cov-2. Nature Microbiology, 5, 536-544. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41564-020-0695-z. [ Links ]

Gould, C. E., Gerolimatos, L. A., Beaudreau, S. A., Mashal, N., & Edelstein, B. A. (2018). Older adults report more sadness and less jealousy than young adults in response to worry induction. Aging & Mental Health, 22 (4), 512-518. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1277975. [ Links ]

Horton, R. (2020). Offline: Covid-19a reckoning. The Lancet, 395 (10228), Article 935. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30669-3. [ Links ]

Hsieh, Y. H., King, C. C., Chen, C. W. S., Ho, M. S., Hsu, S. B., & Wu, Y. C. (2007). Impact of quarantine on the 2003 sars outbreak: A retrospective modeling study. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 244 (4), 729-736.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.09.015. [ Links ]

Jiao, W. Y., Wang, L. N., Fang, S. F., Jiao, Y., Pettoello-Mantovani, M., & Somekh, E. (2020). Behavioral and emotional disorders in children during the covid-19 epidemic. The Journal of Pediatrics, 221, 264-266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2020.03.013. [ Links ]

Kim, S., & Kim, S. (2018). Exploring the Determinants of Perceived Risk of Middle East Respiratory Syndrome (MERS) in Korea. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15 (6), Article 1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15061168. [ Links ]

Lasheras, I., Gracia-García, P., Lipnicki, D., Bueno-Notivol, J., López-Antón, R., de la Cámara, C., Lobo, A., & Santabárbara, J. (2020). Prevalence of Anxiety in Medical Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Rapid Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (18), Article 6603. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17186603. [ Links ]

Li, G., Wang, L., Cao, C., Fang, R., Cao, X., Chen, C., Elhai, J. D., & Hall, B. J. (2020). Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and executive dysfunction among children and adolescents: A Latent Profile Analysis. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 19 (3), 228-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijchp.2019.07.001. [ Links ]

Li, H., Liu, S. M., Tang, S. L., & Tang, C. K. (2020). Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): Current status and future perspective. International Journal of Antimicrobial Agents, 55 (5), Article 105951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijantimicag.2020.105951. [ Links ]

Li, S., Wang, Y., Xue, J., Zhao, N., & Zhu, T. (2020). The Impact of COVID-19 Epidemic Declaration on Psychological Consequences: A Study on Active Weibo Users. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (6), Article 2032. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17062032. [ Links ]

Lima, C. K. T., Carvalho, P. M. M., Lima, I. A. A. S., Nunes, J. V. A. O., Saraiva, J. S., De Souza, I., Lima, C. G., & Neto, M. L. R. (2020). The emotional impact of Coronavirus 2019-nCoV (new Coronavirus disease). Psychiatry Research, 287, Article 112915. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112915. [ Links ]

Liu, N., Zhang, F., Wei, C., Jia, Y., Shang, Z., Sun, L., L. Wu, Z., S., Zhou, U., Wang, Y., & Liu, W. (2020). Prevalence and predictors of PTSS during COVID-19 Outbreak in China Hardesthit Areas: Gender differences matter. Psychiatry Research, 287, Article 112921. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112921. [ Links ]

McDonald, R. (1999). Test theory: A unified approach. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [ Links ]

Mizumoto, K., Kagaya, K., & Chowell, G. (2020). Early epidemiological assessment of the transmission potential and virulence of 2019 Novel Coronavirus in Wuhan City: China, 2019-2020. BMC Medicine, 18, Article 217. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-01691-x. [ Links ]

Molero, M. M., Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Gázquez, J. J., Simón, M. M., & Martos, Á. (2018). Burnout Risk and Protection Factors in Certified Nursing Aides. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 15 (6), Article 1116. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15061116. [ Links ]

Montemurro, N. (2020). The emotional impact of COVID-19: from medical staff to common people. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 87, 23-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.03.032. [ Links ]

Nguyen, H. C., Nguyen, M. H., Do, B. N., Tran, C. Q., Nguyen, T. T. P., Pham, K. M., Pham, L. V., Tran, K. V., Duong, T. T., Tran, T. V., Duong, T. H., Nguyen, T. T., Nguyen, Q. H., Hoang, T. M., Nguyen, K. T., Pham, T. T., Ynag, S. H., Chao, J. C., & Duong, T. V. (2020). People with Suspected COVID-19 Symptoms Were More Likely Depressed and Had Lower Health-Related Quality of Life: The Potential Benefit of Health Literacy. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9 (4), Article 965. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9040965. [ Links ]

Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Molero, M. M., Martos, Á., & Gázquez, J. J. (2020). Threat of COVID-19 and emotional state during quarantine: Positive and negative affect as mediators in a cross-sectional study of the Spanish population. Plos One, 15 (6), e0235305. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235305. [ Links ]

Pietrzak, R. H., Maruff, P., Woodward, M., Fredrickson, J., Fredrickson, A., Krystal, J., Southwick, S. M., & Darby, D. (2012). Mild worry symptoms predict decline in learning and memory in healthy older adults: A 2-year prospective cohort study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20 (3), 266-275. http://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182107e24. [ Links ]

Pulopulos, M. M., Hidalgo, V., Puig-Pérez, S., & Salvador, A. (2018). Psychophysiological response to social stressors: Relevance of gender and age. Psicothema, 30 (2), 171-176. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2017.200. [ Links ]

Real Decreto 463. (2020, march 14). Por el que se declara el estado de alarma para la gestión de la situación de crisis sanitaria ocasionada por el COVID-19, de 18 de marzo de 2020, 25944 a 25945. [ Links ]

Ro, J. S., Lee, J. S., Kang, S. C., & Jung, H. M. (2017). Worry experienced during the 2015 Middle East Respiratory Syndrome (MERS) pandemic in Korea. Plos One, 12 (3), e0173234. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173234. [ Links ]

Ruiz-Hernández, J. A., Moral-Zafra, E., Llor-Esteban, B., & Jiménez-Barbero, J. A. (2020). Influence of Parental Styles and Other Psychosocial Variables on the Development of Externalizing Behaviors in Adolescents: A Systematic Review. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 11 (1), 9-21. https://doi.org/10.5093/ejpalc2018a11. [ Links ]

Sanz, J. (2001). Un instrumento para evaluar la eficacia de los procedimientos de inducción de estados de ánimo: la Escala de Valoración del estado de Ánimo (EVEA) [An instrument to assess the efficacy of mood induction procedures: the "Mood Rating Scale" (MRS)]. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 27 (111), 71-110. [ Links ]

Shim, E., Tariq, A., Choi, W., Lee, Y., & Chowell, G. (2020). Transmission potential and severity of COVID-19 in South Korea. International Journal of Infectious Diseases, 93, 339-344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijid.2020.03.031. [ Links ]

Sim, K., Chan, Y., Chong, P., Chua, H., & Soon, S. (2010). Psychosocial and coping responses within the community health care setting towards a national outbreak of an infectious disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 68 (2), 195-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.04.004. [ Links ]

Skodzik, T., Zettler, T., Topper, M., Blechert, J., & Ehring, T. (2016). The effect of verbal and imagery-based worry versus distraction on the emotional response to a stressful in-vivo situation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 52, 51-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.03.003. [ Links ]

Sohrabi, C., Alsafi, Z., O’Neill, N., Khan, M., Kerwan, A., Al-Jabir, A., Iosifidis, C., & Agha, R. (2020). World health organization declares global emergency: A review of the 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID-19). International Journal of Surgery, 76, 71-76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2020.02.034. [ Links ]

Soriano, J. G., Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Molero, M. M., Tortosa, B. M., & González, A. (2019). Benefits of psychological intervention related to stress and anxiety: Systematic review and meta-analysis. European Journal of Education and Psychology, 12 (2), 191-206. https://doi.org/10.30552/ejep.v12i2.283. [ Links ]

Ventura-León, J. L., & Caycho, T. (2017). El coeficiente Omega: un método alternativo para la estimación de la confiabilidad [The Omega coefficient: an alternative method for estimating reliability]. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud, 15, 625-627. [ Links ]

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C., & Ho, R. (2020). Immediate Psychological Responses and Associated Factors during the Initial Stage of the 2019 Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Epidemic among the General Population in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (5), Article 1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729. [ Links ]

Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownbridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 (3), 644-651. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.3.644. [ Links ]

Wuthrich, V. M., Johnco, C. J., & Wetherell, J. L. (2015). Differences in anxiety and depression symptoms: Comparison between older and younger clinical samples. International Psychogeriatrics, 27 (9), 1523-1532. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610215000526. [ Links ]

Zandifar, A., & Badrfam, R. (2020). Iranian mental health during the covid-19 epidemic. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 51, Article 101990. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.101990. [ Links ]

Zhai, P., Ding, Y., Wu, X., Long, J., Zhong, Y., & Li, Y. (2020). The epidemiology, diagnosis and treatment of COVID-19. International Journal of Antimicrobial Agents, 55 (5), Article 105955. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijantimicag.2020.105955. [ Links ]

Zhou, X. (2020). Psychological crisis interventions in sichuan province during the 2019 novel coronavirus outbreak. Psychiatry Research, 286, Article 112895. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112895. [ Links ]

Declaration of data availability: All relevant data are within the article, as well as the information support files.

How to Cite: Pérez-Fuentes, M. C., Molero Jurado, M. M., Martos Marínez, A., Simón Márquez, M. M. & Gázquez Linares, J. (2021). Mood and Affective Balance of Spaniards Confined by COVID19: A Cross-Sectional Study. International Journal of Psychological Research, 14 (1), 55-65. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.4765

Received: June 10, 2020; Revised: October 05, 2020; Accepted: November 04, 2020

Corresponding author: María del Carmen Pérez-Fuentes. Email: mpf421@ual.es

Conflict of interests:

The authors have declared that there is no conflict of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License