SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.16 número2Comparación de la identificación de expresiones faciales emocionales en rostros completos y solo en las regiones de los ojos y la boca por parte de adultos jóvenes indios: un estudio exploratorio.Validación de la versión española del Massachusetts General Hospital-Sexual Functioning Questionnaire (MGH-SFQ) mediante medidas de excitación sexual en hombres índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


International Journal of Psychological Research

versión impresa ISSN 2011-2084

int.j.psychol.res. vol.16 no.2 Medellín jul./dic. 2023  Epub 10-Oct-2023

https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.6498 

Research article

FamFac - A Database of Famous Faces for Psychology Experiments

FamFac - Una base de datos de caras famosas para experimentos de psicología

Fábio Monteiro1  2   

Paulo Rodrigues3 

Isabel M. Santos2 

Pedro Bem-Haja4 

Pedro J. Rosa5 

1 Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive Behavioral Intervention (CINEICC), Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal.

2 William James Center for Research, Department of Education and Psychology, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal.

3 Sport, Health & Exercise Research Unit (SHERU), Department of Psychology and Education, University of Beira Interior, Covilhã, Portugal.

4 CINTESIS@RISE, Department of Education and Psychology, University of Aveiro, Aveiro, Portugal.

5 Digital Human-Environment Interaction Lab (HEI-Lab), Lusófona University, Lisbon, Portugal.


Abstract.

Introduction.

High variation in the low-level proprieties of visual stimuli and varying degrees of familiarity with famous faces may have caused a bias in the results of investigations that tried to disentangle the processes involved in familiar and unfamiliar face processing (e.g., temporal differences in the detection of the first event-related potentials specialized in face processing may have been caused by different methods of controlling variance in the low-level proprieties of visual stimuli).

Objective.

To address these problems, we developed a freely available database of 183 famous faces whose low-level proprieties (brightness, size, resolution) have been homogenized and the level of familiarity established.

Method.

The brightness of the stimuli was standardized by a custom-developed algorithm. The size and the resolution of the pictures were homogenized in Gimp. The familiarity level of the famous faces was established by a group of 48 Portuguese college students.

Results.

Our results suggest that the brightness of each image did not differ significantly from the mean brightness value of the stimuli set, confirming the standardizing ability of the algorithm. Forty-one famous faces were classified as highly familiar.

Main findings and implications.

This study provides two important resources, as both the algorithm and the database are freely available for research purposes. The homogenization of the low-level features and the control of the level of familiarity of the famous faces included in our database should ensure that they do not elicit confounding effects such as the ones verified in past studies.

Keywords: Face Processing; Face Recognition; Control of Low-Level Features; Methodology.

Resumen.

Introducción.

La existencia de una gran variación en las propiedades de bajo nivel de estímulos visuales y la ocurrencia de diversos grados de familiaridad con rostros famosos pueden haber causado un sesgo en los resultados de las investigaciones que intentaron desentrañar los procesos involucrados en el procesamiento de rostros familiares y desconocidos (por ejemplo, las diferencias temporales en la detección de los primeros potenciales relacionados con eventos especializados en el procesamiento de rostros puede ser explicada por diferentes métodos para controlar la variación en las propiedades de bajo nivel de los estímulos visuales).

Objetivo.

Para mitigar estos problemas, desarrollamos una base de datos de 183 caras famosas, disponible gratuitamente, cuyas propiedades de bajo nivel (brillo, tamaño, resolución) fueron homogeneizados y el nivel de familiaridad medido.

Método.

El brillo de los estímulos fue estandarizado por un algoritmo personalizado. El tamaño y la resolución de las imágenes fueran homogeneizadas en Gimp. El nivel de familiaridad de los rostros famosos fue medido por un grupo de 48 estudiantes universitarios portugueses.

Resultados.

Nuestros resultados sugirieron que el brillo de cada imagen no difiere significativamente del valor de brillo medio del conjunto de estímulos. Cuarenta y un rostros famosos fueron clasificados como altamente familiares. Principales implicaciones. Este estudio proporciona dos recursos importantes, ya que tanto el algoritmo como la base de datos están disponibles gratuitamente para fines de investigación. Los procedimientos de homogeneización deben garantizar que los estímulos incluidos en la base de datos no provoquen efectos de confusión como los verificados en estudios anteriores.

Palabras Clave: Procesamiento facial; reconocimiento facial; control de propiedades de bajo nivel; metodología.

1. Introduction

People tend to view the ability to recognize and identify an individual face as a straightforward process. However, face recognition is a complex phenomenon. Several models have been developed to explain the mechanisms behind face recognition. For instance, the interactive activation and competition model (Burton et al., 1999) and the sequential model of face recognition (Bruce & Young, 1986) suggest that to successfully recognize a familiar face, one has to perceptually process the face, encode its visual proprieties, and create an integrated representation of its configural/holistic characteristics. Then, this integrated representation has to be matched with traces of visual features and semantic/episodic information stored in long-term memory. On the other hand, the Face-Space theory states that faces are represented on a multidimensional space and that each dimension of this space corresponds to a different face feature (face shape, hair color, and length). According to this theory, each known face is represented by a unique point in space. The precise location of a face is determined by the value that each of its features receives in the scale of each dimension. The perceptual difference between two faces is determined by the distance between their locations in space (Valentine et al., 2016).

Past investigations that studied the mechanisms that underlie face recognition suggested that different processes are involved in familiar and unfamiliar face recognition1 (Stacey et al., 2005). For instance, familiar faces are recognized more quickly and accurately than unfamiliar faces (Ramon et al., 2011; Stacey et al., 2005). Additionally, recognizing a familiar face activates a set of brain regions -that store semantic, episodic, and emotional information about a specific familiar person- that are not engaged in unfamiliar face processing (Johnstone & Edmonds, 2009; Natu & O’Toole, 2011). On the contrary, unfamiliar face recognition is achieved by visually processing early structural representations (Bruce & Young, 1986). Familiar and unfamiliar face recognition are also susceptible to different detrimental factors unfamiliar face recognition is impaired by poor lighting conditions, and changes in pose, expression, context, and viewpoint. However, these variables do not have a significant detrimental effect on familiar face recognition (Longmore et al., 2017; Natu & O’Toole, 2011).

This suggests that unfamiliar face processing is more reliant on pictorial information, which becomes less important as faces become more familiar. Additionally, it has been suggested that the internal features of the face (eyes, nose, mouth) are more relevant than external features (hair, ears, face contour) in familiar face recognition (Johnstone & Edmonds, 2009). No particular advantage has been attributed to external or internal features in unfamiliar face recognition (even though a few studies suggested that external features are more important to unfamiliar face recognition) (Bruce et al., 1999).

Several face categories -famous faces (Gosling & Eimer, 2011; Nessler et al., 2005), personally familiar faces (Leibenluft et al., 2004), and experimentally learned faces (Dubois et al., 1999)- have been used to assess differences in familiar and unfamiliar face processing. The use of famous faces to assess this phenomenon has the advantage of providing rich pictorial, semantic, and episodic information to participants that may facilitate the activation of semantic memory traces during face recognition. Additionally, the continued exposure to famous faces, through media, allows for robust recognition across viewpoints, lighting conditions, and poses (Natu & O’Toole, 2011). However, Ramon et al. (2011) argue that famous faces may bias the results of familiar face processing experiments for at least two reasons: (1) there may be a wide range of exposure to each famous face across participants, and (2) the images of famous faces used in these experiments are often ‘iconic’ pictures of celebrities, which can lead to image-based recognition instead of familiar face recognition (e.g., the participants may recognize the picture without being familiar with the famous face). Image-based recognition relies on different mechanisms than familiar face recognition. Thus, to avoid such a confounding effect, investigations that use famous faces to assess differences in familiar and unfamiliar face processing should select famous faces that are highly familiar to all participants and should not select iconic pictures that can be recognized based on other pictorial features instead of the famous face. Additionally, these investigations must take into account that the level of familiarity of each famous face is constrained by geographic and socio-cultural variables: (1) a highly familiar famous face in the UK is not necessarily universally recognized in France; (2) a face universally recognized in a sample of elderly adults may not be recognized by all participants in a sample of teenagers (Lima et al., 2021). The need to consider the influence of variables, such as the nationality of the participants, led to the development of several databases of famous faces in recent years. For instance, Bizzozero et al. (2007) developed a famous face database and collected normative data for the Italian population. Lima et al. (2021) and Marful et al. (2018) conducted similar endeavors for the Portuguese and Spanish population, respectively. These studies followed extremely rigorous norms and procedures, which ensure that their stimuli can be used to study the underpinnings of familiar face recognition with reduced bias in samples with similar characteristics to theirs. However, these studies did not control the low-level proprieties of their images.

The existence of a high degree of variance in the low-level proprieties (brightness, contrast, movement) of the stimuli used to evaluate differences between familiar and unfamiliar face processing may also bias the results of investigations that seek to disentangle the mechanisms that underpin these two phenomena (Andrews et al., 2015; Bainbridge & Oliva, 2015; McCourt & Foxe, 2003). For instance, some authors (Knebel et al., 2008; Willenbockel et al., 2010) suggested that temporal differences in the detection of the first event-related potentials (ERP) associated with face processing may be caused by different methods of controlling variance in the lowlevel proprieties of experimental stimuli. Additionally, this variance causes an increase in the latency of neuronal responses in the primary visual cortex (Brannan et al., 1998), and in the preliminary phases of thalamic processing (Heap et al., 2018). On the contrary, controlling brightness and contrast histograms, which can be achieved by equating the means and standard deviations of brightness and contrast distributions and by matching the number of pixels at each brightness and contrast level, reduces the variation in neuronal responses in the primary visual cortex (Bainbridge & Oliva, 2015; Willenbockel et al., 2010). For instance, Park et al. (2015) used fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) to assess the impact of varying several properties of visual stimuli on several regions along the occipitaltemporal cortex. They found that the response of V1, an early visual area, is significantly smaller if the spectral energy of the stimuli is equated, and their brightness histograms present a skew value of 0. Additionally, salient low-level features may also cause a confounding effect in pupillometry -high spatial frequencies are associated with a smaller pupil size (Cocker et al., 1994)- and oculometry studies -stimuli with salient low-level features receive more fixations- (Orquin & Loose, 2013). Thus, investigations that seek to evaluate high-level cognitive functions, such as familiar and unfamiliar face recognition, must ensure that the variance in the low-level proprieties of their experimental stimuli is homogenized between and within experimental conditions. This will ensure that detected effects are explained by high-level properties of the stimuli, and not by possible confounding effects modulated by an elevated degree of variance in their low-level proprieties (Knebel et al., 2008; Lakens et al., 2013; Willenbockel et al., 2010).

In this article, we present a database comprising famous faces, whose variance in the low-level proprieties (brightness, size, resolution) was homogenized. Additionally, the familiarity of the famous faces was assessed by a sample of Portuguese college students. The database is available at https://osf.io/x3vsy/. The complete set of images can be freely downloaded, transformed, and used for research purposes.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

The sample of this study consisted of 48 unpaid volunteers who attended the first year of the undergraduate course in Psychology at a university in Portugal. The minimum sample size needed to get reliable familiarity scores was based on previous studies that evaluated the same phenomenon (Gosling & Eimer, 2011; Nessler et al., 2005). Our study was advertised through word of mouth. The sample was collected by convenience the participants were directly recruited at the campus of the University. Each participant signed an informed consent form prior to the data collection session. All participants (41 females, 7 males; aged: 17-30 years; mean age: 19.02 years) were of Portuguese nationality and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. This study was approved by the Ethics and Deontology Committee of the University of Aveiro (N40-CED/2019, approved on January 22nd, 2020).

2.2 Stimuli

The database consists of 183 famous faces. These images were selected and downloaded from the Wikimedia Commons website. Every selected image was licensed under a Creative Commons License, which enables the free distribution of copyrighted work (UNESCO, 2018). The selected famous faces were considered to be widely known by the general Portuguese public. The database includes Portuguese and foreign actors/actresses, musicians, athletes, TV personalities, politicians, scientists, etc. Each image was converted to greyscale, resized (397 397 pixels), and rotated (to ensure that the nasal bone presented a 0 degree angle with the horizontal axis of the image) in GIMP (v2.10). The images were converted to Portable Network Graphics (PNG). Their resolution was also homogenized (every image presents a 96dpi 96dpi resolution). An oval mask was applied to every image to hide the maximal amount possible of external features of each face (hair, ears, face contour), without hiding any of its internal features (eyes, nose, mouth). Additionally, the brightness of the images was homogenized by a custom-algorithm developed in Matlab. To achieve this, our algorithm estimated the brightness of each image (sum of the grey value of each pixel of the image divided by the number of pixels of the image) and the mean brightness of the entire set of images. Then, the brightness of each image was subtracted from the mean brightness of the entire set. This difference was then applied to the grey value of each pixel of the respective images. This ensured that the brightness of each image did not differ significantly from the mean brightness of the entire set. Some examples of the final versions of the famous faces are presented in Figure 1. Our algorithm can be freely used for research purposes and downloaded at https://github.com/PauloJFSFRodrigues/lowlevel-features. The algorithm can be applied to other sets of images to homogenize their brightness values. A detailed description of the features of the algorithm will be presented in another article.

2.3 Procedure

A cross-sectional design was used to assess the familiarity level of the famous faces included in the FamFac database. Data collection procedures took place at the university. A single group of 48 participants evaluated the familiarity level of all of the famous faces included in the FamFac database in a single session. The participants completed a task programmed on E-Prime (v2.0) to assess the familiarity level of the famous faces. Each participant completed the task individually on a PC. At the beginning of the session, the investigators presented verbal instructions on how to complete the task. The task did not contain any practice trials. Throughout the task, each famous face included in the FamFac database was sequentially presented to the participants in random order. In each trial, a famous face was displayed at the center of the screen and a textbox was presented below the famous face. The participants were asked to type some personal information in the textbox if they were able to identify the famous face (e.g., name, or other unique piece of information about the person). We considered that a famous face was correctly identified if the personal information provided by the participants corresponded to the famous face that was depicted (correct responses received a score of 1). Otherwise, the response received a score of 0.

2.4 Data Preparation and Statistical Analyses

2.4.1 Familiarity Scores

The familiarity score of every famous face was calculated by multiplying the proportion of correct responses by 100. In other words, to compute the familiarity score of each face we divided the number of correct responses provided by all participants by the total number of participants (48) and multiplied the resulting score by 100. Thus, the familiarity score of the famous faces could range from 0 to 100. We used the same cutoff points employed by Gosling and Eimer (2011) and Nessler et al. (2005) to classify a face as highly familiar. These authors deemed a face as highly familiar if it received a mean familiarity score above 75 on the scales that they used in their studies.

2.4.1 Power Analysis

The number of stimuli (famous faces) needed to carry the inferential analyses was calculated based on a priori power analyses computed with G*Power (version 3.1.9.7; Faul et al., 2007). An a priori power analysis was computed for each inferential statistical test that we conducted (a one-sample t-test, a Mann-Whitney U test, an independent samples t-test, a Kruskal-Wallis H test, and a between-subjects one-way ANOVA). As no previous study has assessed the same relationships that we evaluated in this investigation, we used a medium effect size (d = .5 for tests in which two means were compared, and f = .25 for analyses in which the means of 3 groups are compared) and an alpha value of .05 to compute each power analysis (Prajapati et al., 2010). The highest result of the power analyses (obtained for the between-subjects one-way ANOVA) suggested that a total sample of 159 famous faces was required to achieve a statistical power of .80.

2.4.3 Statistical Analysis

The data collected in this experiment was prepared and analyzed in Microsoft Excel 2016 and IBM SPSS (v28). A one-sample t-test was employed to evaluate if the brightness of each image did not differ significantly from the mean brightness of the entire set.

As mentioned previously, we applied an oval mask to each famous face to remove the maximal amount possible of external features without hiding its internal features. However, the amount of external features that we were able to remove varied from face to face due to several factors (e.g., viewpoint, face size). One of the factors that contributed the most to these differences was the variability in the hairstyles of the famous faces included in the database. For example, we were able to completely remove the hair of some celebrities with short hair, but we had to keep some hair in the case of celebrities who wore bangs. To evaluate if the amount of hair depicted in each picture had a significant impact on the familiarity scores of the famous faces, we split the database into three categories: faces without hair (65 images), faces in which a small amount of hair was kept -e.g., pictures in which we were unable to remove the widow’s peak, a V-shaped point in the hairline in the center of the forehead- (88 images), and faces in which a great amount of hair was kept -e.g., famous faces with bangs- (30 images). A between-subjects one-way ANOVA and a Kruskal-Wallis H test were computed to evaluate if the amount of hair depicted in the pictures had a significant effect in the familiarity scores of the famous faces. Three Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests and a Levene’s test were computed to evaluate if the three groups of images presented normal distributions, and to check if the variance in these three categories was approximately equal. The result of the latter test suggested that the variances of the three groups were not homogenous, F (2, 180) = 3.50, p = .03. The results of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests suggested that the group of images in which a small amount of hair was kept, D(88) = .07, p = .20, and the images in which a great amount of hair was kept, D(30) = .13, p = .20, presented an approximately normal distribution. However, the famous faces that did not presented any hair did not present a normal distribution, D(65) = .11, p = 0.05. As both assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity were not met, we followed the recommendation of Fife-Schaw (2006) and computed a between-subjects one-way ANOVA (parametric test) and a Kruskal-Wallis H test (non-parametric test) to evaluate if the amount of hair depicted in the pictures had a significant effect in the familiarity scores of the famous faces. As both these tests supported similar conclusions, we choose to present the results of the parametric test, because this test is more robust, which decreases the probability of making a Type-I error.

Note. First Row: Angela Merkel, Ben Affleck, Bill Gates, and Bradley Cooper; second row: Donald Trump, José Mourinho, Madonna, and Usain Bolt; third row: Angelina Jolie, Barack Obama, Cobie Smulders, and Daniel Craig; fourth row: Zlatan Ibrahimovi, Scarlett Johansson, Tom Hanks, and Kim Kardashian.

Figure 1 Examples of Famous Faces Included in the Database 

Additionally, we wanted to ascertain if the participants were more familiarized with Portuguese/national famous faces or foreign/international famous faces. Thus, a Mann-Whitney U test and an independent-samples t-test were carried to assess the effect of type of nationality on the familiarity scores of the famous faces. Considering that the subset of Portuguese/national famous faces and the subset of international famous faces presented unequal sizes (N Portuguese famous faces = 17 vs. N International famous faces = 166), a Levene’s test was carried out to evaluate if the variance in the familiarity scores of the two subsets was approximately equal. The result of this test suggested that the variance in the familiarity scores of both subsets was approximately equal, F (1, 181)= 1.82, p = .18. Two Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were carried to evaluate if the familiarity scores of the subset of Portuguese/national famous faces and the subset of international famous faces presented a normal distribution. The familiarity scores of the subset of Portuguese/national famous faces presented a normal distribution, D(17) = .13, p = .20. However, the same was not verified for the familiarity scores of the international famous faces, D(166) = .08, p = .02. As the assumption of normality was not met, we took the recommendation of Fife-Schaw (2006) into account once again and performed an independent samples t-test (parametric test) and a Mann-Whitney U test (non-parametric test). As both tests yielded similar results, we decided to present the results of the parametric test.

Effect sizes were estimated with Cohen’s d. The benchmarks suggested by Cohen (1988) were used to interpret the magnitude of these effects: small effect (d = .2), medium effect (d = .5), and large effect (d = .8).

3. Results

The brightness of each image was successfully homogenized by the algorithm developed on Matlab. Our results suggested that the brightness of each image did not differ significantly from the mean brightness of the entire stimuli set (M = 154.08, 95% CI [-.06, 0.06]), t(182) < .001, p > .99, d < .001, which indicates a small effect size. The brightness values of the stimuli are presented in the supplementary materials (see Table S1). An xlxs file with this data can be downloaded at https://osf.io/x3vsy/.

As previously mentioned, we used the cutoff point employed by Gosling and Eimer (2011) and Nessler et al. (2005)) to classify a face as highly familiar. Taking these metrics into account, 41 out of the 183 famous faces were highly familiar to our participants. The familiarity scores of the 183 famous faces are displayed in Table 1. This information can also be downloaded at https://osf.io/x3vsy/ in xlxs format.

The results of the between-subjects one-way ANOVA suggested that the amount of hair depicted in each image of the FamFac did not have a significant effect in the familiarity scores of the famous faces, F (2, 180) = .25, p = .78, η 2 < .01. The results of the post-hoc tests revealed that the familiarity scores of the images without hair (M = 47.88, 95% CI [41.22, 54.54]) did not differ significantly from the familiarity scores of the images in which a small amount of hair was kept (M = 50.42, 95% CI [45.11, 55.72]), or the images in which a great amount of hair was kept (M = 51.46, 95% CI [39.58, 63.35]) (all p > .05).

Additionally, the result of the independent-samples ttest, t(181) = 1.81, p = .07, d = .46, suggested that the familiarity scores of Portuguese/national famous faces (M = 60.83, 95% CI [48.57, 73.10]), and the familiarity scores of international famous faces (M = 48.55; 95% CI [44.43, 52.66]), did not differ significantly, although there was a tendency for Portuguese famous faces to have a higher familiarity score than international famous faces.

4. Discussion

The results of previous studies suggested that processing familiar and unfamiliar faces elicits different behavioral and electrophysiological responses (Bentin & Deouell, 2000; Gosling & Eimer, 2011; Ramon et al., 2011; Stacey et al., 2005). However, the results of electrophysiological and eye-tracking studies that seek to assess the different mechanisms involved in familiar and unfamiliar face processing may be confounded by several exogenous variables that exert a significant influence on the way that pictorial and structural codes of familiar and unfamiliar faces are processed and retrieved, such as the existence of significant variation in the low-level proprieties of the stimuli -e.g., brightness, contrast- (Knebel et al., 2008; Willenbockel et al., 2010); or the existence of varying degrees of familiarity with the faces presented to the participants (Ramon et al., 2011). Thus, investigations interested in disentangling the processes involved in familiar and unfamiliar face processing must ensure that each face presented to the participants is universally recognized, and that variation in the low-level proprieties of the stimuli is kept to a minimum. With these objectives in mind, we developed a database comprising 183 images of famous faces originally licensed under a Creative Commons License, whose low-level proprieties (brightness, size, resolution) were homogenized. The complete set of images is available at https://osf.io/x3vsy/ and can be freely downloaded, transformed, and used for research purposes.

We developed an algorithm in Matlab to automatically equate the brightness of the images included in the FamFac. The algorithm successfully homogenized the brightness of the set of stimuli -the brightness of each image did not differ significantly from the mean brightness of the entire set. Some studies suggested that some ERP components (e.g., P1 and N1) are sensitive to variations in the physical properties of stimuli, such as brightness (Schettino et al., 2016; Schindler et al., 2018). This effect is minimized when the variation between the low-level features of the stimuli is controlled for (Schettino et al., 2016). Considering that the brightness of each image included in the FamFac database was homogenized, it is possible to suggest that these stimuli should not elicit such confounding electrophysiological modulations. The algorithm used to equate the brightness of the images can be freely downloaded from https://github.com/PauloJFSFRodrigues/lowlevel-features. This code can be used to homogenize the brightness of other images sets.

As previously stated, investigations that use famous faces to study familiar face processing should ensure that the selected famous faces are highly familiar to all participants (Ramon et al., 2011). For instance, to guarantee that the famous faces that were going to be included in their main experiment were highly familiar, Gosling and Eimer (2011) asked eight participants to name and state the profession of a large pool of 129 famous persons. The famous faces that were included in their main experiment were those that were explicitly identified by at least six participants, which can be translated into a familiarity score of 75 on the scale that was used in our study. The same cutoff point was used by Nessler et al. (2005); these authors included a famous face in the set of stimuli that was used in their main experiment if its mean rating was higher than 3 (their familiarity scale ranged from 1 to 4). Taking these metrics into account, 41 out of the 183 famous faces included in the FamFac database were considered highly familiar by our sample. Our results also suggested that the familiarity scores of Portuguese/national famous faces and foreigner famous faces did not differ significantly, although approaching statistical significance (p = .07). Additionally, the subset of Portuguese/national famous faces consisted only of 17 stimuli. Thus, these results must be treated with caution. Ramon et al. (2011) suggested that varying degrees of familiarity with a given famous face between participants may lead to some additional variability in electrophysiological and ocular correlates elicited by this said famous face. Our results suggest that, in the case of the Portuguese population, there may be some advantage in using national famous faces to evaluate the mechanisms involved in familiar face processing, given that this subset presented higher familiarity scores than the subset comprising foreign faces. However, this difference did not achieve statistical significance.

Table 1 Familiarity Scores of the Famous Faces Included in the Database 

Famous Face Familiarity Score Famous Face Familiarity Score
Cristiano Ronaldo 100.00 John Travolta 70.73
Ed Sheeran 100.00 Kim Kardashian 70.73
Dwayne Johnson 97.56 Sandra Bullock 70.73
Barack Obama 97.56 António Costa 68.29
Justin Bieber 97.56 José Mourinho 68.29
Miley Cyrus 95.12 David Schwimmer 68.29
Albert Einstein 95.12 Jeremy Renner 68.29
Donald Trump 95.12 Penelope Cruz 68.29
Taylor Swift 92.68 Salvador Sobral 65.85
Robert Pattinson 92.68 Sylvester Stallone 65.85
Jennifer Aniston 92.68 Chris Pratt 65.85
Zac Efron 92.68 Gigi Hadid 65.85
Leonardo DiCaprio 92.68 Meghan Markle 65.85
Diogo Amaral 90.24 Blake Lively 65.85
Jackie Chan 90.24 Vera Kolodzig 65.85
Emma Watson 90.24 Joana de Verona 63.41
Katy Perry 90.24 Madonna 63.41
Demi Lovato 90.24 Natalie Dormer 63.41
Emma Stone 87.80 Sarah Jessica Parker 63.41
Lady Gaga 85.37 Daniel Craig 63.41
Angelina Jolie 85.37 Margot Robbie 60.98
Kristen Stewart 85.37 Heath Ledger 60.98
Selena Gomez 85.37 Luís Figo 58.54
Ricardo Araújo Pereira 82.93 Hugh Jackman 58.54
Scarlett Johansson 82.93 Paul Rudd 58.54
Cara Delevingne 82.93 Kaley Cuoco 58.54
Meryl Streep 82.93 Victoria Guerra 56.10
Chris Hemsworth 82.93 Ezra Miller 56.10
Rupert Grint 80.49 Vince Vaughn 56.10
Anne Hathaway 80.49 Tom Hanks 56.10
Dakota Johnson 80.49 Bradley Cooper 56.10
Ellen DeGeneres 80.49 Alec Baldwin 56.10
Ian Somerhalder 80.49 Ellie Goulding 53.66
Maisie Williams 78.05 Danny Glover 53.66
Eddie Redmayne 78.05 Cate Blanchett 53.66
Michelle Obama 78.05 Ana Sofia Martins 51.22
Débora Monteiro 75.61 Vladimir Putin 51.22
Rui Unas 75.61 Felicity Jones 51.22
Kanye West 75.61 Chris Rock 51.22
Julianne Moore 75.61 Pope Francis 51.22
Channing Tatum 75.61 Sofia Vergara 51.22
Virgílio Castelo 73.17 Eva Longoria 48.78
Angela Merkel 73.17 Christine Baranski 48.78
Jason Momoa 73.17 Kate Winslet 48.78
Ryan Gosling 73.17 Amy Adams 48.78
Steve Jobs 70.73 Arnold Schwarzenegger 46.34
Jason Statham 70.73 Claire Holt 46.34
Kate Walsh 46.34 Miranda Kerr 24.39
Maya Rudolph 46.34 Kate Mara 24.39
Glenn Close 46.34 Jason Sudeikis 24.39
Charlize Theron 43.90 Michael Douglas 24.39
Jordana Brewster 43.90 George Bush 24.39
Ben Kingsley 43.90 Sarah Hyland 24.39
Olivia Wilde 43.90 Sacha Baron Cohen 24.39
Kevin Costner 43.90 Carmen Electra 21.95
António Guterres 41.46 Henry Cavill 21.95
Octavia Spencer 41.46 Dan Reynolds 21.95
Ashley Olsen 41.46 Stephen Merchant 21.95
Ben Affleck 41.46 Howie Mandel 21.95
Christian Bale 41.46 Clint Eastwood 21.95
Jennifer Morrison 41.46 Evan Rachel Wood 19.51
Kevin Bacon 41.46 Elon Musk 19.51
Leighton Meester 41.46 LeBron James 19.51
Dylan McDermott 39.02 Manuela Ferreira Leite 17.07
Robert De Niro 39.02 Bill Murray 17.07
Rachel Weisz 39.02 Monica Belluci 17.07
Ewan McGregor 36.59 Dakota Fanning 14.63
Victoria Justice 36.59 Ricky Martin 14.63
Diane Kruger 36.59 Doutzen Kroes 14.63
Giselle Bündchen 36.59 Christine Lagarde 14.63
Susan Sarandon 36.59 Usain Bolt 14.63
Zlatan Ibrahimovi 36.59 Claire Foy 14.63
José Rodrigues Dos Santos 34.15 Ivanka Trump 14.63
Chris Evans 34.15 Sienna Miller 14.63
Cobie Smulders 34.15 Cynthia Nixon 14.63
Mila Kunis 34.15 Carminho 12.20
Bill Gates 34.15 Zidane 12.20
Maggie Gyllenhaal 34.15 Theresa May 12.20
Ashley Judd 31.71 Rafael Nadal 12.20
Eric Bana 31.71 Christina Hendricks 12.20
Meg Ryan 31.71 Shaquille O’Neal 12.20
Tina Fey 31.71 Pep Guardiola 9.76
Eric Stonestreet 31.71 Lake Bell 9.76
Toni Kroos 29.27 Kevin Durant 9.76
Jennifer Connelly 29.27 John Carpenter 9.76
JK Simmons 29.27 Muhammad Ali 7.32
Brendan Fraser 29.27 Sarah Silverman 7.32
Conor McGregor 29.27 King Juan Carlos 7.32
Demi Moore 26.83 Ashlee Simpson 4.88
Sophie Turner 26.83 Ronnie O’Sullivan 2.44
Sigourney Weaver 26.83 Jean-Claude Van Damme .00
Naomie Harris 24.39

For the sake of clarity, we will now discuss some of the limitations of this study. First of all, we would like to highlight that the number of famous faces with a familiarity score above 75 is relatively limited. We believe that this was due to the difficulty in finding a substantial set of high-quality images whose copyright license enabled us to freely use and transform them. For the same reason, the images included in the database have a low resolution (96dpi 96dpi). The oval mask that was superimposed on the faces to control for external and background features may also have harmed the familiarity scores of the famous faces. Nevertheless, we decided to keep the oval masks due to the high variability of the original images. We believe that keeping the original images without applying the oval mask would have introduced some bias in the familiarity scores because the participants could use the external features of the faces or other pictorial elements of the images to help them recognize the famous face, which could lead to image-based recognition instead of identity-based recognition. This was particularly true for our category of stimuli -famous faces- as some external features, such as hairstyles, could have been associated with the celebrity depicted. However, in the future, it would be interesting to evaluate if the familiarity scores would improve if the famous faces were presented without the oval masks. Importantly, the familiarity scores of this set of stimuli were assessed with a Portuguese sample. Thus, investigations that want to assess familiar face recognition with samples with different nationalities should evaluate the familiarity scores of the faces included in this database for these samples. Additionally, we recommend that investigations that intend to use this database to assess face recognition with Portuguese samples should choose their set of stimuli from the 41 famous faces that received a familiarity score higher than 75. The algorithm that we developed on Matlab successfully controlled the brightness of the images. However, several other low-level features (e.g., image complexity, movement, orientation, contrast) can cast a similar confounding effect in investigations that use electrophysiological or eye-tracking methods to evaluate high-level cognitive functions (Bainbridge & Oliva, 2015; Dragoi et al., 2000; Kamitani & Tong, 2006). Nonetheless, due to constrained nature of the present stimuli set (faces with ovals hiding all external and background features), we believe that those effects are limited in this set. Still, future studies with similar aims using stimuli with less constrained characteristics should ensure that the low-level features previously mentioned are also homogenized between their stimuli. The development of algorithms like the one we developed to control the brightness of visual stimuli could facilitate this procedure.

5. Conclusions

We believe that the materials made freely available for research purposes within this study (a famous face dataset anda brightness homogenizing algorithm) are particularly useful resources for studies of face processing, but can also be used in other studies which use visual stimuli and may need to control for low-level features. Furthermore, the original images wecollectedwere licensed under a Creative Commons License. Thus, any authors that wish to use this database can use the available stimuli, and transform them to fit the specific needs of their investigations.

References

Andrews, T. J., Watson, D. M., Rice, G. E., & Hartley, T. (2015). Low-level properties of natural images predict topographic patterns of neural response in the ventral visual pathway. Journal of Vision, 15 (7), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.7.3Links ]

Bainbridge, W. A., & Oliva, A. (2015). A toolbox and sample object perception data for equalization of natural images. Data in Brief, 5, 846-851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2015.10.030Links ]

Bentin, S., & Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17 (1-3), 35-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432900380472Links ]

Bizzozero, I., Lucchelli, F., Saetti, M. C., & Spinnler, H. (2007). “whose face is this?”: Italian norms of naming celebrities. Neurological Sciences, 28 (6), 315-322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072007-0845-6Links ]

Brannan, J. R., Solan, H. A., Ficarra, A. P., & Ong, E. (1998). Effect of luminance on visual evoked potential amplitudes in normal and disabled readers. Optometry and Vision Science: Official Publication of the American Academy of Optometry, 75 (4), 279-283. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199804000-00025Links ]

Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77 (3), 305- 327. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.xLinks ]

Bruce, V., Henderson, Z., Greenwood, K., Hancock, P. J., Burton, A. M., & Miller, P. (1999). Verification of face identities from images captured on video. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5 (4), 339. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.5.4.339Links ]

Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Hancock, P. J. (1999). From pixels to people: A model of familiar face recognition. Cognitive Science, 23 (1), 1-31. https:// doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2301_1Links ]

Cocker, K. D., Moseley, M. J., Bissenden, J. G., & Fielder, A. R. (1994). Visual acuity and pupillary responses to spatial structure in infants. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 35, 2620- 2625. [ Links ]

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [ Links ]

Dragoi, V., Sharma, J., & Sur, M. (2000). Adaptation induced plasticity of orientation tuning in adult visual cortex. Neuron, 28 (1), 287-298. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(0000103-3Links ]

Dubois, S., Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., Bodart, J. M., Michel, C., Bruyer, R., & Crommelinck, M. (1999). Effect of familiarity on the processing of human faces. Neuroimage, 9, 278-289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0409Links ]

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2), 175-191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146Links ]

Fife-Schaw, C. (2006). Levels of measurement. In G. M. Breakwell, S. Hammond, C. Fife-Schaw, & J. A. Smith (Eds.), Research Methods in Psychology (pp. 147-157). Sage Publications. [ Links ]

Gosling, A., & Eimer, M. (2011). An event-related brain potential study of explicit face recognition. Neuropsychologia, 49 (9), 2736-2745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.025Links ]

Heap, L. A., Vanwalleghem, G., Thompson, A. W., FavreBulle, I. A., & Scott, E. K. (2018). Luminance changes drive directional startle through a thalamic pathway. Neuron, 99 (2), 293-301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.06.013Links ]

Johnston, R. J., & Edmonds, A. J. (2009). Familiar and unfamiliar face recognition: A review. Memory, 17 (5), 577-596. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902976969Links ]

Kamitani, Y., & Tong, F. (2006). Decoding seen and attended motion directions from activity in the human Visual Cortex. Current Biology, 16 (11), 1096-1102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.003Links ]

Knebel, J. F., Toepel, U., Hudry, J., Le Coutre, J., & Murray, M. M. (2008). Generating controlled image sets in cognitive neuroscience research. Brain Topography, 20 (4), 284-289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-008-0046-5Links ]

Lakens, D., Fockenberg, D. A., Lemmens, K. P., Hamm, J., & Miden, C. J. (2013). Brightness differences influence the evaluation of affective pictures. Cognition and Emotion, 27 (7), 1225-1246. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.781501Links ]

Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children. Biological Psychiatry, 56 (4), 225-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017Links ]

Lima, D., Pinto, R., & Albuquerque, P. B. (2021). Recognition and naming test of the Portuguese population for national and international celebrities. Behavior Research Methods, 53 (6), 2326-2337. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01572-yLinks ]

Longmore, C. A., Santos, I. M., Silva, C. F., Hall, A., Faloyin, D., & Little, E. (2017). Image dependency in the recognition of newly learnt faces. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70 (5), 863-873. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1236825Links ]

Marful, A., Díez-Álamo, A. M., Plaza-Navas, S., & Fernandez, A. (2018). A normative study for photographs of celebrities in spain. Plos One, 13 (5), e0197554. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197554Links ]

McCourt, M. E., & Foxe, J. J. (2003). Brightening prospects for “early” corticol coding of perceived luminance. Journal of Vision, 3 (9), 49-56. https://doi.org/10.1167/3.9.424Links ]

Natu, V., & O’Toole, A. J. (2011). The neural processing of familiar and unfamiliar faces: A review and synopsis. British Journal of Psychology, 102 (4), 726-747. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02053.xLinks ]

Nessler, D., Mecklinger, A., & Penney, T. B. (2005). Perceptual fluency, semantic familiarity and recognition-related familiarity: An electrophysiological exploration. Cognitive Brain Research, 22 (2), 265-288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.03.023Links ]

Orquin, J. L., & Loose, S. M. (2013). Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 144 (1), 190-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.003Links ]

Park, S., Konkle, T., & Oliva, A. (2015). Parametric coding of the size and clutter of natural scenes in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 25 (7), 1792-1805. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht418Links ]

Prajapati, B., Dunne, M., & Armstrong, R. (2010). Sample size estimation and statistical power analyses. Optometry Today, 16 (7), 10-18 [ Links ]

Ramon, M., Caharel, S., & Rossion, B. (2011). The speed of recognition of personally familiar faces. Perception, 40 (4), 437-449. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6794Links ]

Schettino, A., Keil, A., Porcu, E., & Müller, M. (2016). Shedding light on emotional perception: Interaction of brightness and semantic content in extrastriate visual cortex. NeuroImage, 133, 341-353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.020Links ]

Schindler, S., Schettino, A., & Pourtois, G. (2018). Electrophysiological correlates of the interplay between low-level visual features and emotional content during word reading. Scientific Reports, 8, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598018-30701-5Links ]

Stacey, P. C., Walker, S., & Underwood, J. D. (2005). Face processing and familiarity: Evidence from eye-movement data. British Journal of Psychology, 96 (4), 407-422. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X47422Links ]

UNESCO. (2018). The Creative Commons licenses. https://en.unesco.org/open-access/creative-commons-licensesLinks ]

Valentine, T., Lewis, M. B., & Hills, P. J. (2016). Facespace: A unifying concept in face recognition research. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69 (10), 1996-2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.990392Links ]

Willenbockel, V., Sadr, J., Fiset, D., Horne, G. O., Gosselin, F., & Tanaka, J. W. (2010). Controlling low-level image properties: The SHINE toolbox. Behavior Research Methods, 42 (3), 671-684. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.671Links ]

1Unfamiliar face recognition refers to the process of recognizing a previously unknown face. For example, when that face is presented among a set of face stimuli and later must be recognized among new distractor faces; or when participants need to decide whether two photographs of unknown faces display the same person or two different people (Johnstone & Edmonds, 2009).

Declaration of data availability: All relevant data are within the article, as well as the information support files.

6. Support Files The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://osf.io/x3vsy/, Table S1.

7. Author Contributions Conceptualization, I.M.S., P.B-H., P.R., P.J.R.; methodology, I.M.S., P.B-H., P.R., P.J.R., and F.M.; software, P.R., and F.M.; formal analysis, F.M., P.B., and P.B-H.; investigation, F.M., and P.R.; data curation, F.M., P.R., and P.B-H.; writingoriginal draft preparation, F.M.; writingreview and editing, F.M., I.M.S., P.B-H, P.R., and P.J.R.; visualization, F.M., and P.R; supervision, I.M.S., and P.R.; project administration, I.M.S.; funding acquisition, I.M.S., and P.R.. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

8. Funding This work was supported by national funds through FCT Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia, I.P., with the project PTDC/PSI-GER/31082/2017.

9. Institutional Review Board Statement The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics and Deontology Committee of the University of Aveiro (N40CED/2019, approved on January 22nd, 2020).

10. Data Availability Statement The database presented in this study is openly available at https://osf.io/x3vsy/. The algorithm that we developed to homogenize the brightness of experimental stimuli can be found and downloaded from https://github.com/PauloJFSFRodrigues/lowlevel-features.

Received: August 03, 2022; Revised: October 06, 2022; Accepted: July 05, 2023

Corresponding author: Fábio Monteiro. Email: uc2021194081@student.uc.pt

Conflict of interests:

The authors have declared that there is no conflict of interest.

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License