SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.5 issue1Neuromuscular Alterations Associated with COVID-19. A Scientific Literature ReviewBorderline Personality Disorder, Early Adverse Experiences, and Cognitive Biases: A Systematic Review author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Revista de investigación e innovación en ciencias de la salud

On-line version ISSN 2665-2056

Rev. Investig. Innov. Cienc. Salud vol.5 no.1 Medellín Jan./June 2023  Epub Apr 11, 2023

https://doi.org/10.46634/riics.134 

Review article

Rehabilitation programs of executive functions in young and middle adulthood based on computational systems: a scoping review

Programas de rehabilitación de las funciones ejecutivas en la adultez joven y media basados en sistemas computacionales: una revisión de alcance

Carolina Robledo-Castro1  * 
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-8494-5305

Piedad Rocio Lerma Castaño2 
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9546-8436

Luis Gerardo Pachón Ospina3 
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6516-3711

1 Departamento de Pedagogía y Mediaciones Tecnológicas; Universidad del Tolima; Ibagué; Colombia.

2 Programa de Fisioterapia; Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud; Fundación Universitaria María Cano; Neiva; Colombia.

3 Gestión Digital del Aprendizaje; Universidad EAFIT; Medellín; Colombia.


Abstract

Introduction:

The use of computational systems has ventured into different healthcare areas, such as rehabilitation and stimulation of cognitive processes. To this date, it is possible to identify some reviews collecting studies on the efficacy and effects of those programs in groups such as older adults, children, and teenagers; there is a lack of academic literature giving an account of young and middle-aged adults.

Objective:

To identify empirical studies that measured the feasibility and effect of computer-based stimulation and rehabilitation programs for cognitive functions in young and middle-aged adults.

Materials and methods:

The PRISMA ScR (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Extension for Scoping Reviews) extension was used as a base for a scoping review, as suggested by Cochrane Collaboration. Five databases -Science Direct, Scopus, Springer, PubMed, and Taylor & Francis- were used to trace information. The data registry and synthesis of the results was carried out independently by two reviewers.

Results:

896 registries were found between 2015 and 2022, of which 91 met the eligibility principles, which evaluated the effects of programs based on computational systems on executive functions on young and middle-aged adults.

Conclusion:

Most of the interventions based on computational systems showed to be feasible and had moderate to significant effects on executive functions in young and middle-aged adults.

Keywords: Cognitive rehabilitation; executive functions; computational systems; systematic review; clinical trials; adulthood

Resumen

Introducción:

El uso de los sistemas computacionales ha incursionado de forma notable en diferentes áreas de la salud, como la rehabilitación y estimulación de los procesos cognitivos. Si bien a la fecha se pueden identificar algunas revisiones que recopilan estudios sobre la eficacia y efectos de estos programas en grupos como adultos mayores, niños y adolescentes, existe poca literatura orientada a la adultez joven y la adultez media.

Objetivo:

Identificar estudios empíricos que midieron la viabilidad y el efecto de los programas de estimulación y rehabilitación de las funciones ejecutivas basados en computadora en la población adulta joven y media.

Materiales y métodos:

Se realizó revisión de alcance basado en la extensión PRISMA ScR (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Extensión for Scoping Reviews) sugerido por Cochrane Collaboration. Para el rastreo de información se seleccionaron cinco bases de datos: Science Direct, Scopus, Springer, PubMed y Taylor & Francis. El proceso de registro de datos y la síntesis de los resultados fue realizada por dos revisores de forma independiente.

Resultados:

Se encontraron 896 registros desde el 2015 al 2022, de los cuales 91 cumplieron los principios de elegibilidad, en los que se evaluaron los efectos de programas basados en sistemas computacionales sobre las funciones ejecutivas en adultos jóvenes y adultos medios.

Conclusión:

La mayoría de las intervenciones basadas en sistemas computacionales mostraron ser viables y tener efectos favorables de moderados a significativos sobre las funciones ejecutivas de adultos jóvenes y medios.

Palabras clave: Rehabilitación cognitiva; funciones ejecutivas; sistemas computacionales; revisión sistemática; ensayos clínicos; adultez

Introduction

Executive functions (EF) are higher-order cognitive processes associated with processing in the prefrontal areas of the brain [1] responsible for efficiently guiding goal-directed behavior. EFs allow the subject to plan, monitor, evaluate performance, adapt to new situations, and solve complex problems in which there has been no prior learning [2]. In general, EFs are cognitive processes that guide, coordinate, review, and evaluate behavior in achieving goals [3,4].

Currently, there is no consensus on the definition and classification of EF; therefore, the evaluation and training approach depends on the theoretical models that support them. One of the most recognized models has been the proposal by Miyake et al. [5], who, through a confirmatory factorial analysis, defined working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control as central nuclei of executive functions, cognitive processes from which others of greater complexity would derive, such as reasoning, problem-solving problems, and planning. Other theoretical models include others (EF), such as verbal fluency [6], planning [7], decision-making [8], and some attentional control processes [9].

Beyond explanatory theoretical models, it is a reality that Executive Functions can present impairment in various clinical pictures, such as syndromes associated with damage to the prefrontal cortex, especially in the dorsolateral, medial, and orbitofrontal areas; the involvement of each one of these areas is expressed in clinical pictures with different alterations of executive functioning [10,11].

EF impact is a characteristic of a large number of health disorders in young and middle adults with some early-onset neurodegenerative disorders, neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., autism, attention deficit disorder, and intellectual disability), mood disorders, psychoactive substance use disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder, stroke, genetic disorders such as Huntington's disease and Gilles de la Tourette syndrome, as well as other diseases such as cancer and HIV [12].

Among the processes of cognitive intervention for the clinical population and the healthy population, the rehabilitation or stimulation paradigms can be recognized. The first focuses on cognitive processes affected by the disease, injury, or neurodegenerative condition. Stimulation, on the other hand, provides opportunities for developing and promoting the capacities of cognitive processes that have not yet been consolidated. In other words, while rehabilitation is based on the principles of restitution, compensation, and substitution, stimulation is based on the principles of transference and generalization [12,13].

Concerning these programs, the literature suggests that intervention schemes should be aimed at improving cognitive processes and their neurological bases under the principle of neuroplasticity, producing a reorganization in brain structure and functioning [12]. The studies also propose that the activities of said programs should aim at processes rather than task training, so that in addition to generating close transfer, associated with improvement in untrained tasks that measure the same cognitive component, they also succeed in causing a far transfer, that is, an improvement in untrained tasks that measure related cognitive components [14].

One of the alternatives for rehabilitation and cognitive stimulation has been programs based on computer systems. Studies on this type of program have shown a promising panorama in different age groups. However, some of the findings restrict their effectiveness to early ages or older adults; other studies position these technologies among the new directions of cognitive training, with effects throughout life, albeit with varying degrees of efficiency [15-18]. For this reason, research evaluating the feasibility and effects of computerized cognitive training programs remains a growing need in a way that allows exposing the feasibility of incorporating these programs into clinical practice [15,19].

Reviews that evaluate the effectiveness of computerized cognitive training programs on executive functions in different population groups have shown contradictory results, to a certain extent associated with characteristic methodologies of these studies [20]; most of these reviews have focused on the elderly [21,22] with sequelae of cerebrovascular accident [23], and adults with cognitive impairment. These studies have found improvements in general cognitive function in older adults [21]; however, the effect on executive functions has not been studied. Only one review was conducted on the average adult [24]; the authors restricted the search to interventions with more than 12 weeks and found only one article with eligibility criteria. In their work, they concluded that computerized cognitive training in midlife demonstrated lasting effects on general cognitive function with 12 weeks of training and memory after 24 weeks of training [24].

Based on this background and in the absence of reviews on the topic of interest, it is proposed to carry out a scoping review with the aim of providing a general description of the evidence collected so far on cognitive stimulation and rehabilitation programs for executive functions, based on computer systems applied in young and middle adulthood.

Methods

Protocol and registration

The selected methodology was the scoping review, since it is better suited to the proposed objective, which is to provide a general description of a particular topic to identify knowledge gaps and support new studies [25]. The scoping review is formulated as a stepwise process of five phases: 1) identify the research question; two) Identify relevant studies; 3) select studies; 4) graph the data; and 5) summarize and report the results [26]. The present study's authors formulated an a priori protocol following the PRISMA ScR (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Extension for Scoping Reviews) extension for scoping reviews checklist [27], and it was reviewed by an external peer specialized. Said protocol is not publicly available, but it is available on demand through the correspondence author's email.

Eligibility Criteria

The established inclusion criteria were research articles published between 2015 and 2022, experimental studies (controlled clinical trials, open clinical trials, and quasi-experimental studies), articles that study computerized programs for the stimulation or rehabilitation of executive functions that involved the population between 18 and 60 years of age, written in English, and that included one or two measures of executive functions. The exclusion criteria were established as follows: short articles, summaries or protocols, reviews and meta-analyses, articles that do not evaluate executive functions, interventions that are not based on the use of computer systems, and studies in populations under 18 or over 60 years of age.

Information sources

To identify potentially relevant documents, the following bibliographic databases, resource sources, and digital platforms were searched: Science Direct, Scopus, Springer Link, PubMed, and Taylor & Francis. The research team designed and tested the search strategies and reviewed them with a specialized professional. In the review protocol, the authors established the central concepts for the construction of the search chain of the studies: 1) Executive functions, 2) Rehabilitation and cognitive stimulation, 3) Computer systems, and 4) young and middle adulthood. These categories were translated into English and reviewed in the ERIC thesauri (recognized terms in education and health from the Educational Resources Information Center). Based on these central concepts identified and the review carried out in said thesauri, a search chain was built that included logical operators, which was executed in October 2022 in the databases selected for the search of the articles. The final search strategy for each database is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Search strategy for each database or digital platform. 

Database (total records) search algorithm automatic filters
Science direct (96) Year:2015-2022Title, abstract, keywords: (( "Executive function" OR "executive function" OR "Executive functioning") AND ("cognitive training" OR "Cognitive Stimulation" OR rehabilitation ) AND ( computerized OR based-computer ) -The "Years" filter was applied (2015 to 2022)
-The "Type of article" filter was applied by selecting the option "Research articles" *
Springers (243) ("Executive function" OR "executive function" OR "Executive functioning ") AND ("cognitive training" OR "Cognitive Stimulation" OR rehabilitation) AND (computerized OR based-computer) -The "Data published in" filter was applied (2015 and 2022)
-The "Type of content" filter was applied, selecting the "Article" option *
Pubmed (148) (("Executive function"[Title/Abstract] OR "Executive function" [Title/Abstract] OR "Executive functioning" [Title/Abstract]) AND ("cognitive training" [Title/Abstract] OR "Cognitive Stimulation" [Title /Abstract] OR "rehabilitation" [Title/Abstract]) AND ("computerized" [Title/Abstract] OR "computer-based" [Title/Abstract])) AND (2015:2022[ pdat ]) -The "Publication date" filter was applied to select works published between 2015 and 2022.
-The "Type of article" filter was applied, selecting the options "Clinical trial" and "Randomized controlled trial" *
Taylor & Francis (184) [[All: "executive function"] OR [All: "executive function"] OR [All: "executive functioning"]] AND [[All: "cognitive training"] OR [All: "cognitive stimulation"] OR [All: rehabilitation]] AND [[All: computerized] OR [All: computer-based]] AND [Article Type: Article] AND [Publication Date: (01/01/2015 TO 12/31/2022)] -The "Publication date" filter was applied to select works published between 2015 and 2022.
-The “Source type” filter was applied, selecting the option “Scientific journals” and “Theses and doctoral dissertations” *
Scopus (225) TITLE-ABS-KEY (("Executive function" OR "executive function" OR "Executive functioning") AND ("cognitive training" OR "Cognitive Stimulation" OR rehabilitation) AND (computerized OR based-computer)) AND PUBYEAR > 2014 AND PUBYEAR < 2023 AND PUBYEAR > 2014 AND PUBYEAR <2023 AND (LIMIT-TO (DOCTYPE, "ar"))) -The "Year" filter was applied to select documents published between 2015 and 2022.
-The "Type of document" filter was applied to select the options "Article" and "Conference article" *

Note. Excluding from the search results the documents of review articles, meta -analyses, brief reports, editorials, publications, case reports, conference information, book reviews, encyclopedia, brief communications, protocol documents, reference work, conference article , book, encyclopedia and conference proceedings, audio and video works, newspapers, brochures and encyclopedias, reports and bibliographic reviews, case studies, reports, reviews of search results.

Selection process

The first author searched the different databases, implemented automatic filters, and eliminated duplicate articles. Next, the first and second authors screened the title and abstract of each article based on the established inclusion and exclusion criteria. The third author carried out a second review of the articles that had passed the first filters. Subsequently, the full text of each article was reviewed independently by the three authors to verify whether or not the eligibility criteria were met; the articles in doubt were reviewed collectively to reach an agreement between the authors, and, in exceptional cases, they were sent for arbitration to an external adviser.

Data extraction

Based on previous reviews, the authors adapted a documentary analysis matrix in which the data extracted from each article were recorded: identification data, place of publication, year of publication, sample size, mean age of the participants, clinical conditions of participants, methodological design, study objectives, characteristics of the training and type of activities, characteristics of the implemented computer system, executive functions studied, measurement instruments, primary results, secondary results, and conclusions.

Results

Once the search chain was executed, 896 records distributed in the different databases were identified. By applying the automatic filters of the search tool (time range, complete articles, original articles, English language, disciplinary areas of interest) and excluding duplicates, the titles and abstracts were screened to proceed to a review of the full text. After assorting these filters, the articles included for the review and analysis comprised 91 studies (see Figure 1). In Appendix A, you can consult the descriptive matrix in which the main characteristics of each study are summarized.

Note 1. * The study did not assess executive functions; ** The population was not young adults or middle adults; *** The intervention was not computerized.

Note 2: Science Direct (96), Scopus (225), Springer Link (243), PubMed (148) and Taylor & Francis (184). When reviewing the title and abstract, it was identified that the articles did not correspond to empirical studies (review articles, opinion articles, short articles, conferences, clinical protocols, posters, they do not present research results), they did not study cognitive training programs based on computer systems or did not investigate in the selected population.

Figure 1. Flowchart 

Most of the studies published in the last eight years were developed in North America (34%), followed by Spain (9.1%), Germany (6.8%), the United Kingdom, and Italy (5.6%). Only one of the studies was carried out in a Latin American country (Cuba). Of the total of 88 studies, 32 were in young adults with a mean age of 22.8 years (Standard Deviation 2.4), and 55 were in middle adults with a mean age of 42.9 years (Standard Deviation 9.2).

To present the results, young adults (between 18 and 30 years) and middle-aged adults (between 31 and 60 years) were discriminated against. As seen in Table 2, most of the studies on young adults were conducted in a healthy population, with most of the participants being university students. Most of the clinical population of this population range had a diagnosis of schizophrenia or depressive disorders. The studies in average adults were mainly in the clinical population, where the most frequent pathologies were disorders associated with the schizophrenia spectrum (21.8%) and multiple sclerosis (18.2), to a lesser extent, consumption disorders of substances (10.9%) and cerebrovascular accidents.

Table 2 Clinical characteristics of the study population. 

Characteristics of the study population Total Young adult Middle adult
Frequency (%) N = 91 N = 30 N = 61
Healthy population 27 (29.7) 23(76.7) 4 (6.6)
Multiple sclerosis 13 (14.3) 13(21,3)
Schizophrenia spectrum 11 (12.1) 4 (13.3) 7 (11.5)
Acquired brain damage 8 (8.7) 8 (13.1)
Disorders associated with substance use 8 (8.7) 1 (3.3) 7 (11.5)
Depressive disorders 8 (8.7) 5 (16.7) 3 (4.9)
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder 4 (4.4)
Cancer survivors 3 (3.3) 3 (10) 4 (6.6)
Mild cognitive impairment 3 (3.3) 3 (4.9)
Anxiety disorders 3 (3.3) 3 (4.9)
Human immunodeficiency virus 2 (2.2) 2 (3.3)
Huntington's disease 2 (2.2) 2 (3.3)
Obsessive compulsive disorder 1 (1,1) 1 (1.6)
Chronic fatigue syndrome 1 (1,1) 1 (1.6)

Note: The frequency of the studies is discriminated by young adult and middle adult population, with the respective percentage weighted by population in parentheses.

Regarding the executive functions studied, some of the studies reviewed (24.2%) were based on theoretical models that contemplate the executive function construct in a unified way, making a general reference to the ability to regulate different cognitive, behavioral, and affective functions [28]. In contrast, the rest of the studies assessed executive functions independently.

Similar to the findings of previous systematic reviews conducted in other age groups [29], the most studied executive function was working memory (62.5%), both in clinical and healthy populations. At the same time, some executive cognitive domains were more representative in specific age ranges, such as attention and processing speed, addressed in 58.2% and 38.2% of the studies in middle-aged adults, respectively, but not as a representative in young adults, where most of the studies focused on working memory (69.6%) and, to a lesser extent, attention (18.2%) and inhibitory control (15.1%). Other domains, such as planning and verbal fluency, were studied exclusively in average adults (see Table 3).

Table 3 Executive functions studied by age group. 

Frequency (%) Total Young adult Middle adult
N = 91 N = 30 N = 61
Executive functioning 31 (33.3) 7 (20.6) 24 (39.7)
Work memory 55 (60.4) 20 (66.7) 35 (57.1)
Attention 33 (35.4) 6 (17.6) 28 (44.4)
Cognitive flexibility 14 (14.6) 3 (98.8) 11 (17.5)
Inhibitory control 16 (16.7) 6 (17.6) 10 (15.9)
Processing speed 27 (28.1) 4 (11.7) 23 (36.5)
Planification and organization 6 (6.3) 6 (9.5)
Problem resolution 6 (6.3) 2 (5.8) 4 (6.3)
Emotional regulation 6 (6.3) 2 (5.8) 4 (6.3)
Verbal fluency 6 (6.3) 6 (9.5)
Decision making 4 (4.1) 2 (5.8) 2 (3.2)

Note: The frequency of the studies is discriminated by young adult and middle adult population, with the respective percentage weighted by population in parentheses.

Among the rehabilitation or cognitive stimulation programs based on computer systems, the review allowed us to identify both commercial programs and novel programs designed by the same authors or research groups, with the former used in 60% of the investigations. 52% of the interventions implemented game-based activities, ranging from gamified versions of classic exercises to the design of serious games for specific purposes or commercial video games. 16% of the programs were based on classical tasks, and 4.5% on multimodal programs that included other types of interventions such as physical activity and meditation, in addition to computerized training. The most used device was the computer (57.9%); other devices were tablets (11.3%), smartphones (2.2%), and video game consoles (2.2%), although 26% of the studies did not specify the type of device used.

Most research on young adults was done on healthy people, while most of the research on mid-adults was done on people with clinical conditions. The most representative findings by population group are described below.

Schizophrenia

The most frequent computerized cognitive training program in the studies of the population with the schizophrenia spectrum was the commercial platform Brain HQ, which was included in 7 of the investigations. Studies showed mixed results, some reporting significant effects of training on working memory [30-32], processing speed [30,32-35], attention [32,30], and problem-solving [31], also proving to be a good complement to pharmacological treatment [34]. However, some studies did not find cognitive benefits of the Brain HQ intervention in cognitive functions such as processing speed [36] and the combination with COGPACK [37]; the authors indicate that this lack of effects could be due to the age of the participants (older than in other studies) and that the effects of the medications could reduce the effectiveness of the treatment [37]. However, the same authors report good adherence and satisfaction of the participants towards the treatment.

Among the studies carried out with Brain HQ in the schizophrenia population with comorbid depression and anxiety [32], it was found that a predictor of cognitive gain in working memory and attention is the patient's commitment to treatment, as well as the level of cognitive performance at the beginning of the treatment, finding that those who benefited the most from the training were the people who showed the lowest performance in the pretest. On the other hand [30], she points out that a significant predictor was the number of hours of treatment, such that a greater number of hours of training produced larger improvements in working memory and processing speed. In a study evaluating the combination of Brain HQ training and Modafinium pharmacological treatment, the authors found improvements in trained tasks; however, the medication group showed no differential effects from the group receiving cognitive training alone [34]. Auditory-targeted cognitive training with Brain HQ also showed a positive correlation with measures associated with auditory processing speed in this population [35].

Another of the programs studied in the population with schizophrenia was CIRCuiTS, a computerized program for cognitive remediation. Interventions with this program led to better performance in visual memory [38], working memory, and processing speed [39], but showed no effect on attention, reasoning, and problem-solving. However, it was demonstrated to be a protective factor against cognitive decline in this population [39]. A study with the Neuronpersonaltrainer program showed effects on attentional processes, memory, and emotional processing in people with early stages of schizophrenia, although it did not show transfer to daily life functioning and social cognition [40]. Finally, the study by Matsuoka et al. [41] evaluated the neurological changes after training with the cognitive remediation software Jcores VCAT-J; the authors found that this intervention induced microstructural changes in the posterior lobe of the left cerebellum, which were associated with changes in the verbal fluency of the participants.

Multiple sclerosis

In adults with multiple sclerosis, the most widely implemented training program was the Rehacom cognitive rehabilitation software, which was included in 6 studies [42-47]. Studies found improvements in measures of processing speed [42,43,46,48-50], verbal and visuospatial working memory [42-44,46,47,51-53], verbal fluency [44,46,47], visuospatial skills [46], attention [46,47,50,53], and general cognitive performance measured with the SDMT digit and symbol test. However, changes in many of these cognitive processes were not sustained after rehabilitation [45,43], while improvements in attention [42] and processing speed [46] were maintained after months.

The Cogni-track cognitive remediation program, based on computerized working memory exercises, and showing high adherence to treatment, improved working memory in patients with multiple sclerosis. The authors found that the intervention is more effective in programs supported by machine learning algorithms that allow activities to have an adaptive and progressive difficulty [51]. Also, studies in this population were conducted with Neuroracer [48], demonstrating effects on processing speed and psychomotor function. A novel Speed Processing Training (SOPT) program demonstrated a transfer effect close to the processing speed measured with WAIS-III [49]. With the Cogmed program [52], near-transfer effects were observed on working memory and far on several measures of attention, with a secondary effect on mood.

The work of Pérez-Martín et al. [50] evaluated a computerized novel training based on n-back combined with self-training at home to reinforce cognitive strategies; the intervention showed positive effects on attention, working memory, and processing speed, with transfer effects on mood. One study compared the effectiveness of computer-based (Captain's Log, Mindpower), manual, and combined cognitive rehabilitation, but the authors did not find that all three approaches showed effectiveness in rehabilitating working memory, executive function, processing speed, and selective attention in subjects with multiple sclerosis. However, they did not find superiority of one approach over the other, concluding that all three are equally effective [53].

Depression and anxiety

In another population studied, young adults and middle adults were patients with depressive disorders. Most studies found positive effects of different programs on executive functioning in general [54-56] and some specific executive functions. In patients with major depressive disorder, a clinical trial with the Cogniplus program significantly improved attentional processes and strengthened adaptive and socialization skills [57]. In this same population, the computerized game-based program PEAK led to improvements in executive functioning and processing speed with no effect on depressive symptoms [54], and intervention with the Cogmed working memory training platform improved executive function, processing speed, and emotional working memory [55].

Their clinical trial [56] compared goal management training with computerized training. The study showed that the former was superior to the latter in reducing everyday executive dysfunction, with effects on symptom reduction at a six-month follow-up.

In anxiety disorder individual's adaptive n-back training led to improvements in WM, with transfer to the reduction of self-reported worry and anxiety symptoms [58]. In people with emotional disorders related to stress, advanced gaze contingent attention training led to significant improvements in selective attention, while generating several beneficial effects: increased attention to positive over negative information, greater reappraisal ability to regulate negative emotions, and reduced state rumination compared with the control group [59].

In adults with post-traumatic stress, cognitive-affective training with a gamified PEC-SCA did not show a generalized benefit on EF or symptoms [60]. Lumosity showed improved performance for participants who received working memory training and yoga than those who received working memory training alone. The improvement of group 1 participants in working memory skills is possibly due to the additional benefits of yoga [61]. Lumocity was also studied in veteran combatants with post-traumatic stress disorders; home-based cognitive training demonstrated feasibility, adherence, and satisfaction in participants. Such training also led to improvements in self-perceived executive functions, as well as subjective improvement in symptoms of depression post-traumatic stress [62].

Acquired brain damage

In adults with acquired brain injury, we found two clinical trials evaluating the BrainGymmer program: one of the studies showed an improvement in cognitive flexibility after the intervention [63], while a second study carried out by the same authors, although improvements were recognized in some EFs, they occurred both in the intervention group and in the control groups, suggesting that the improvement was due to non-specific training effects [64]. Nor was there any evidence of effects on any cognitive function associated with the intervention with Cognifit in the same population [65].

Another study with the Erica cognitive remediation platform showed improvement in attention and memory capacity in the intervention group, with benefits in mood in patients with ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke [66]. An RCT compared a paper-and-pencil intervention with Rehacom, finding superiority in the latter's effects in favoring sustained attention and working memory [67]. In patients with subacute stroke, a clinical trial found that training of 6 to 10 sessions with virtual reality activities based on tasks of daily living led to improvements in flexibility, inhibition, conceptualization, programming and memory, and global cognition [68].

In patients with stroke, a virtual reality environment for prospective memory rehabilitation showed a significant improvement in prospective memory skills, which remained stable four weeks after treatment [69]. Power AFA software is successful in improving language and cognitive recovery in patients with post-stroke aphasia in the chronic phase [70]. In a study of patients with traumatic brain injury, impaired attention, and executive control, improvements in attention regulation were observed following an intervention with APT - Attention Process Training-Based Cognitive Training [71].

Substance abuse-associated disorders

Different programs based on computer systems have also been investigated in disorders related to the abuse of psychoactive substances. In an intervention with Cogmed, improvements in verbal working memory function were found in patients with histories of drug abuse, but did not show transfer effects to other cognitive processes, nor did it reduce symptoms of drug abuse [72,73].

The effects of the computerized program Curb Your Addiction were studied in patients in the process of rehabilitation for methamphetamine use, finding improvements in working memory in addition to traditional treatment. Additionally, it was shown to reduce clinical symptoms of impulsivity and improve self-regulation [74]. This same program in patients with substance abuse disorder was associated with increased volume in mesolimbic reward regions that coincided with improvements in impulsivity scores [75]. In this same population, an intervention with Lumosity was shown to improve performance in inhibition tasks, with an effect on quality-of-life indicators [76]. And a novel program based on Goal Management Training together with pharmacological treatment showed a significant effect on inhibitory control, with transfer to working memory and planning, as well as a reduction in impulsivity [77].

In recently detoxified alcohol use disorder patients, two versions of a novel n-back task-based program showed improvements in inhibition, with greater effects in the emotional version of training compared to training with neutral stimuli [78]. In adult smokers, a non-commercial computer program showed no significant effect on EF after it was applied [79].

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Three studies on young adults with ADHD were identified. One of them [80] evaluated the effects of a computerized program of progressive attention training (CPAT) and found that the intervention group showed significant improvements in selective-spatial attention, as well as near transfer to executive attention, with effects that were sustained after follow-up, although with no effect on self-reported ADHD symptoms. The other two studies evaluated the Cogmed working memory training platform. In one of them, the training effects were small [81], while the other study showed improvements in working memory tasks, without transfer effect nor differential effects in longer interventions compared to short interventions. However, the participants of the extended version of the intervention demonstrated greater use of strategies and not only of solving tasks [82].

Other clinical conditions

In adults with cognitive impairment, HappyneuronPro generated improvements in executive functioning, increased frontal theta power, and reduced posterior alpha EEG power at two weeks of training [83].

In patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder, an intervention with Cogtrain, a program designed by the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, generated improvements in Executive Functioning and processing speed and contributed to the reduction of cognitive depressive symptoms [84].

Regarding surviving cancer patients, two studies report an evaluation of the feasibility and effects of cognitive training with Brain HQ: Wu et al. [85] found that although the program was shown to be feasible and highly acceptable, it only showed benefits on reaction time, while Von et al. [86] found that both computerized cognitive training programs and remotely administered active attention control computer programs are feasible, as they generate small positive effect size and adherence on cognitive processes exercises.

The pilot study by [87] evaluated a program based on video games called Mycognitivon in cancer survivors with cognitive impairment; the intervention group showed that memory, perception, and motor function had a different evolution in the intervention group, showing less deterioration compared to the control group. An intervention with the HappyneuronPro program in women with breast cancer generated improvements in memory and concentration, verbal learning, and WM, which were maintained after five months [88].

Two studies were found in patients with Huntington's disease. In one of them, a multidisciplinary rehabilitation intervention integrated with aerobic exercises, computerized cognitive training (Neuronation) and dual-task training and sleep hygiene, and nutritional guidance, led to significant improvements in verbal learning and memory, attention, flexibility, cognitive, and processing speed in the intervention group compared to the control group [89]. At the same time, the second study developed with the HappyneuronPro program, while showing adherence in one group of the sample, did not achieve widespread feasibility in the sample as a whole [90].

In two clinical trials [91,92], the effect of computer-based cognitive remediation programs was evaluated in patients with HIV, one of them implemented an intervention with Lumosity [92], and in another, an intervention with PSSCogRehab [20]. Both interventions studied led to reducing the working memory deficit in these patients.

Healthy population

As mentioned above, 4 of the studies were conducted in a healthy population of average adults, while in young adults, 20 were identified, most of them in university students.

Programs based on the n-back paradigm were one of the most frequent to train working memory [93-97] and showed transfer effects to other processes such as attentional control [95], cognitive flexibility [97], inhibition [95,97], dual tasking [94], reaction time [98], and visual short-term memory [95] in healthy adults. In studies with healthy adults, training based on other classical tasks such as the Stroop test [99,100], the Flanker test [100], and other classical tasks [100-102] has also been recognized. Commercial computer training programs such as Lumosity [103-104], Cogmed [105], Mind frontiers [106-108], and Gwakkamole app [109] have been identified as well. Also, novel programs designed by researchers [110-114] have been remarked.

One of the commercial programs based on the n-back paradigm was Brain Twister2, an ascending complexity scheme that generated increasingly complex training demands. Interventions based on this program demonstrated greater near transfer to untrained working memory tasks and far transfer to attentional control [115]. Some authors found that in task-based n-back training, the improvements seemed restricted to tasks like those trained in working memory, but without transfer to other cognitive processes [93,94,116]. While other programs, based on strategies and not on tasks, showed greater transfer to other untrained tasks [96,100].

Three studies used training with the Mind Frontiers serious game-based cognitive stimulation program. Zwilling et al. [106] observed effects of near transfer to working memory tasks and far transfer to perceptual speed tasks in an intervention with this program in healthy young adults, while Baniqued et al. [107] found similar near transfer to working memory tasks and far from processing speed and reaction time. In another study, Mind frontiers showed transfer to decision-making and general executive functioning, with greater effect when combined with other programs such as meditation and high-intensity cardio resistance training [108].

On the other hand, in an intervention with Lumosity, improvements were observed in cognitive flexibility, attention, and reaction times, although these changes were not reflected in evoked potentials [104]. An fMRI study demonstrated that a novel executive control training intervention based on the Flanker task caused an increase in amygdala-IFG connectivity, further showing improvements in inhibitory control, attention, and emotional regulation [100]. A study contrasted a novel program based on classical tasks to train working memory by comparing a linear scheme and one with progressive difficulty. The authors concluded that cognitive training programs should incorporate adaptive task difficulty to enhance the transfer of training gains and maximize the efficiency of task-related brain activity [117].

Some studies that implemented working memory training, such as Cogmed [105] and other non-commercial programs created by the researchers [114,118], showed improvements that were restricted to specific working memory tasks without no other near and far transfer effects. In this vein, training with MultiTask also did not show significant effects on working memory, cognitive flexibility, or inhibition [119]. Another working memory training program such as Neuronation showed effects on trained working memory tasks, but also showed substantial improvements in near transfer tasks to other domains of working memory [120]. Some inhibition training programs, such as Gwakkamole [109] and other novel programs [112,113], did not significantly affect inhibition or transfer to other domains, such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, and planning. While Namratha et al. [101] found that working memory training in healthy average adults achieves greater results when combined with yoga training. Task repetition-based training demonstrated close transfer to similar tasks but no transfer effect to inhibitory control [118].

Discussion

This scoping review aimed to provide an overview of the evidence collected to date on computer-based cognitive stimulation and rehabilitation programs for executive functions applied in young and mid-adulthood. The main finding of this scoping review was that most studies on the use of computer-based cognitive training programs had shown favorable results on executive functions in young and middle adults.

The executive functions of working memory, attention, processing speed, and inhibitory control were the most studied in the population between 18 and 60 years of age. Working memory has been the most studied executive function also in other population groups [93,111]. In this scoping review, in addition to being the most frequent, it is also the executive function that showed the greatest benefits of cognitive training programs mediated by computer systems and in which greater effects of near and far transference were recognized both in healthy and healthy populations, which demonstrated that training in this cognitive domain could benefit the development of other cognitive abilities, such as reasoning skills, mental focus in daily activities, multitasking, and attention.

Rehabilitation and stimulation of other executive functions, such as cognitive flexibility, planning, verbal fluency, inhibitory control, and emotional regulation, were less frequent, and the effects on inhibitory control generally showed more limited effects [33,109]. These results show an important knowledge gap in future research and intervention designs mediated by computer systems.

A large part of the studies in healthy young adults was carried out on university students [98,105,110,112,114-116]. In these, the participants generally belonged to the same university that led the study, situation which can lead to a significant bias since when doing a convenience sampling with a captive population, although the risk of dropout in the study is minimized, it limits the results to the sociodemographic characteristics of that population.

Among the populations with the most studied clinical conditions in young and middle adults were multiple sclerosis [42-50,53], schizophrenia [30-39,41,67], acquired brain injury [63-65,67,69,70], substance use disorders [72-78], and depressive disorders, [32,54-57,78] and a minority of studies were conducted in young adults with Huntington's disease [89,90], stress-related emotional disorders [59-62], and cancer survivors [80,86-88]. Many of these studies evaluated the effects of interventions on the executive functions of sustained attention, concentration, and speed of information processing.

Regarding the effects of the interventions, the review shows varied results and, in some aspects, contradictory. Although most of the studies showed effects of near transference that, for the most part, ranged from moderate to significant, other programs studied did not show the expected effects or showed a very limited transference, especially in the effects of far transference, which indicates that the degree of generalization is limited in these intervention programs [20], as reported by Etherton et al. [105], who in their results found that working memory training with the Cogmed program does not improve performance on unrelated tasks or improve other cognitive abilities. From these results, some researchers conclude that the effect of distant transfer after executive function training is far from clear.

Research has found multiple benefits of integrating computer systems into cognitive stimulation and rehabilitation in adults. The use of online access platforms allows the professional in charge of the intervention to have a wide range of activities to select, manage, and schedule, which permits them to design personalized work sessions and record the evolution of the subject. Some of these platforms have digital test batteries and machine learning modalities that provide "intelligent tutoring" [104], giving instructions, accompaniment, and constant feedback to the subject and the personnel in charge of care. The clinical interventions showed better effects when accompanied by sessions at home; access to the computerized program from multiple devices precisely provides continuity to the clinic from home or another location without requiring special equipment. This characteristic had a favorable impact on retention and adherence to treatment while allowing the optimization of technological and human resources, since it allows individual but simultaneous intervention with several subjects simultaneously.

The computerized machine learning algorithms take the data of the person's performance and learn from this data to adapt to the complexity of the activities and provide a training scheme with ascending and progressive complexity, this being one of the most outstanding and best characteristics results in interventions [51]. Clinical interventions showed better effects when they included game-based, adaptive, challenging activities of increasing complexity and in highly motivating environments [51,58,83,119]. The researchers suggest that future interventions look for ecological validity in the design of the activities, so that the tasks represent the problems that adults face in their daily lives [71,120]. Several studies agreed that the greatest benefits were obtained by people with lower levels of cognitive impairment at the time of the pretest, which may be due to a ceiling effect [32].

Some research found that the improvement in some domains was not due to a specific improvement in that domain, but rather a consequence of the more efficient and strategic use of the cognitive skills learned during training. They also identified that considerably better results are obtained by combining the intervention based on tools and computational skills with training in metacognitive strategies and problem-solving [50,61,100]. Therefore, this may be a promising research field for further study.

Conclusions

Following the above, in most studies, interventions mediated by computer systems are effective in stimulating some executive functions in young and middle adults. However, these findings also demonstrate the need to study computer-based treatments for other clinical and subclinical conditions in young and middle adults with impaired executive functioning that have not been addressed in empirical studies and include follow-up measures in the methodological design of the studies. It is necessary to carry out more studies in young and middle adults with other diagnoses, such as mild cognitive impairment, obsessive-compulsive disorders, anxiety disorder, neurodevelopmental disorders, and intellectual disability, among others, since few studies were identified in the reviewed literature.

Finally, based on the results of this review, it was possible to collect an important corpus of studies on the effects of rehabilitation and stimulation of executive functions in young and middle adults with programs mediated by computer systems. However, the absence of Latin American publications in this population was evident. Therefore, the authors suggest, for future research, the development of a systematic review and meta-analysis that allows the evaluation of the size of the effect, the quality of the studies, and biases. For future studies, the authors suggest that the tasks be designed to train multiple cognitive skills and not specifically focus on task performance.

References

1. Goldstein S, Naglieri JA, Princiotta D, Otero TM. Introduction: A history of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct. In: Handbook of executive functioning. New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media; 2014. p. 3-12. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_1Links ]

2. Portellano Pérez JA, García Alba J. Neuropsicología de la atención, las funciones ejecutivas y la memoria. Madrid: Síntesis; 2014. [ Links ]

3. Goldberg E. El director ejecutivo del cerebro: Una mirada a los lóbulos frontales. In: El cerebro ejecutivo Lóbulos frontales y mente civilizada. Barcelona: Planeta; 2015. p. 37-38. [ Links ]

4. Blair C, Ursache A. A bidirectional theory of executive functions and self-regulation. In: Vohs K, Baumeister R, editores. Handbook of self-regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. p. 300-320. [ Links ]

5. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex «Frontal Lobe» tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734Links ]

6. Flores Lázaro JC, Ostrosky F. Desarrollo neuropsicológico de los lóbulos frontales y funciones ejecutivas. México: El Manual Moderno; 2012 [ Links ]

7. Zelazo PD, Li Q, Müller U. Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, wo. In: Schneider W, Schumann-Hengsteler R, Sodian B, editors. Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, wo. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2005. p. 71-93. [ Links ]

8. Cañabate SC, Alcázar AM. Control ejecutivo, toma de decisiones, razonamiento y resolución de problemas. In: Redolar D, editor. Neurociencia cognitiva. España: Editorial Médica Panamericana; 2013. p. 719-745. [ Links ]

9. Baddeley A, Eysenck MW, Anderson MC. Memoria de trabajo. In: Memoria. 2da edición. Madrid: Alianza. 2009. p. 63-91. [ Links ]

10. Delgado-Mejía ID, Etchepareborda MC. . Rev Neurol. 2013;57 Suppl 1:S95-103. doi: https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.57S01.2013236Links ]

11. Vayas R, Carrera L. Disfunción ejecutiva. Síntomas y relevancia de su detección desde Atención Primaria. Revista Clínica de Medicina de Familia. 2012;5(3):191-7. doi. https://doi.org/10.4321/S1699-695X2012000300007Links ]

12. Portellano JA. Intervención neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas. In: Neuroeducación y funciones ejecutivas. Madrid. CEPE; 2018. p. 117-146. [ Links ]

13. Trápaga CM. Introducción a la estimulación y rehabilitación de las funciones cognitivas. In: Trápaga CM, Pelayo HJ, Sánchez I, Bello Z, Bautista A. Editores. De la psicología cognitiva a la neuropsicología. México: Manual Moderno; 2018. p. 18-59. [ Links ]

14. Liu Q, Zhu X, Ziegler A, Shi J. The effects of inhibitory control training for preschoolers on reasoning ability and neural activity. Sci Rep. 2015;5(1):14200. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/srep14200Links ]

15. Seelye AM, Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Das B, Cook DJ. Application of Cognitive Rehabilitation Theory to the Development of Smart Prompting Technologies. IEEE Reviews in Biomedical Engineering. 2012; 5:29-44. doi: https://doi.org/10.1109/RBME.2012.2196691Links ]

16. Homer BD, Ober TM, Flynn RM. Children and Adolescents’ Development of Executive Functions in Digital Contexts. In: Proceedings of the Technology, Mind, and Society . New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery; 2018 . p. 1-3. (TechMindSociety ’18). doi: https://doi.org/10.1145/3183654.3183696Links ]

17. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, Burns AS, et al. Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010;465(7299):775-8. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09042Links ]

18. Schubert T, Strobach T, Karbach J. New directions in cognitive training: on methods, transfer, and application. Psychological Research. 2014;78(6):749-55. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0619-8Links ]

19. Guerrero G, García A. Plataformas de rehabilitación neuropsicológica: estado actual y líneas de trabajo. Neurología. 2015;30(6):359-66. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrl.2013.06.015Links ]

20. Robledo-Castro, CR, Castillo-Ossa, Corchado, JM. Artificial Cognitive Systems Applied in Executive Function Stimulation and Rehabilitation Programs: A Systematic Review. Arabian journal for science and engineering. 2022;48:2399-2427. 28 doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13369-022-07292-5Links ]

21. Gates NJ, Rutjes AW, Di Nisio M, Karim S, Chong LY, March E, et al. Computerised cognitive training for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019;3:CD012277. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub2Links ]

22. Ten Brinke LF, Best JR, Chan JLC, Ghag C, Erickson KI, Handy TC, et al. The Effects of Computerized Cognitive Training With and Without Physical Exercise on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: An 8-Week Randomized Controlled Trial. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2020;75(4):755-63. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glz115Links ]

23. Yoo C, Yong M hyun, Chung J, Yang Y. Effect of computerized cognitive rehabilitation program on cognitive function and activities of living in stroke patients. J Phys Ther Sci. 2015;27(8):2487-9. doi: https://doi.org/10.1589/jpts.27.2487Links ]

24. Gates NJ, Vernooij RW, Di Nisio M, Karim S, March E, Martínez G, et al. Computerised cognitive training for preventing dementia in people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2019;3:CD012279. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD012279.pub2Links ]

25. Munn Z, Peters MDJ, Stern C, Tufanaru C, McArthur A, Aromataris E. Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2018;18(1):143. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0611-xLinks ]

26. Arksey H, O’Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology. 2005;8(1):19-32. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616Links ]

27. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467-73. doi: https://doi.org/10.7326/M18-0850Links ]

28. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24(1):167-202. doi: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167Links ]

29. Stelzer F, Andés ML, Canet-Juric L, Introzzi I. Working Memory and Fluid Intelligence. A Review of its Relations. Acta de investigación psicológica. 2016;6(1):2302-16. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/s2007-4719(16)30051-5Links ]

30. Bell MD, Laws H, Pittman B, Johannesen JK. Comparison of focused cognitive training and portable “brain-games” on functional outcomes for vocational rehabilitation participants. Sci Rep. . 2018;8(1):1779. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20094-wLinks ]

31. Biagianti B, Fisher M, Howard L, Rowlands A, Vinogradov S, Woolley J. Feasibility and preliminary efficacy of remotely delivering cognitive training to people with schizophrenia using tablets. Schizophr Res Cogn. . 2017;10:7-14. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2017.07.003Links ]

32. Harvey PD, Balzer AM, Kotwicki RJ. Training engagement, baseline cognitive functioning, and cognitive gains with computerized cognitive training: A cross-diagnostic study. Schizophr Res Cogn. . 2020;19:100150. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2019.100150Links ]

33. Piskulic D, Barbato M, Liu L, Addington J. Pilot study of cognitive remediation therapy on cognition in young people at clinical high risk of psychosis. Psychiatry Research . 2015;225(1):93-98. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.10.021Links ]

34. Michalopoulou PG, Lewis SW, Drake RJ, Reichenberg A, Emsley R, Kalpakidou AK, et al. Modafinil combined with cognitive training: Pharmacological augmentation of cognitive training in schizophrenia. European Neuropsychopharmacology . 2015;25(8):1178-89. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.03.009Links ]

35. Tarasenko M, Perez VB, Pianka ST, Vinogradov S, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR, et al. Measuring the capacity for auditory system plasticity: An examination of performance gains during initial exposure to auditory-targeted cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res . 2016;172(1-3):123-30. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.01.019Links ]

36. Mahncke HW, Kim SJ, Rose A, Stasio C, Buckley P, Caroff S, et al. Evaluation of a plasticity-based cognitive training program in schizophrenia: Results from the eCaesar trial. Schizophr Res . 2019; 208:182-189. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.006Links ]

37. Jahshan C, Vinogradov S, Wynn JK, Hellemann G, Green MF. A Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing a “Bottom-Up” and “Top-Down” Approach to Cognitive Training in Schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res . 2019;109:118-25. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.11.027Links ]

38. Reeder C, Huddy V, Cella M, Taylor R, Greenwood K, Landau S, et al. A new generation computerised metacognitive cognitive remediation programme for schizophrenia (CIRCuiTS): a randomised controlled trial. Psychol Med . 2017;47(15):2720-2730. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001234Links ]

39. Palumbo D, Mucci A, Giordano GM, Piegari G, Aiello C, Pietrafesa D, et al. The Efficacy, Feasibility And Acceptability Of A Remotely Accessible Use Of CIRCuiTS, A Computerized Cognitive Remediation Therapy Program For Schizophrenia: A Pilot Study. NDT . 2019;15:3103-3113. doi: https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S221690Links ]

40. Fernandez-Gonzalo S, Turon M, Jodar M, Pousa E, Hernandez Rambla C, García R, et al. A new computerized cognitive and social cognition training specifically designed for patients with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder in early stages of illness: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res . 2015;228(3):501-509. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.007Links ]

41. Matsuoka K, Morimoto T, Matsuda Y, Yasuno F, Taoka T, Miyasaka T, et al. Computer-assisted cognitive remediation therapy for patients with schizophrenia induces microstructural changes in cerebellar regions involved in cognitive functions. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. . 2019; 292:41-6. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.09.001Links ]

42. Messinis L, Nasios G, Kosmidis MH, Zampakis P, Malefaki S, Ntoskou K, et al. Efficacy of a Computer-Assisted Cognitive Rehabilitation Intervention in Relapsing-Remitting Multiple Sclerosis Patients: A Multicenter Randomized Controlled Trial. Behav Neurol . 2017;2017:5919841. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/5919841Links ]

43. Messinis L, Kosmidis MH, Nasios G, Konitsiotis S, Ntoskou A, Bakirtzis C, et al. Do Secondary Progressive Multiple Sclerosis patients benefit from Computer- based cognitive neurorehabilitation? A randomized sham controlled trial. Mult Scler Relat Disord . 2020;39:101932. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2020.101932Links ]

44. Arian Darestani A, Naeeni Davarani M, Hassani-Abharian P, Zarrindast MR, Nasehi M. The therapeutic effect of treatment with RehaCom software on verbal performance in patients with multiple sclerosis. J Clin Neurosci . 2020;72:93-97. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.007Links ]

45. Campbell J, Langdon D, Cercignani M, Rashid W. A Randomised Controlled Trial of Efficacy of Cognitive Rehabilitation in Multiple Sclerosis: A Cognitive, Behavioural, and MRI Study . Neural Plast. 2016;2016:4292585. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/4292585Links ]

46. Naeeni Davarani M, Arian Darestani A, Hassani-Abharian P, Vaseghi S, Zarrindast MR, Nasehi M. RehaCom rehabilitation training improves a wide-range of cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis patients. Appl Neuropsychol Adult .2022;29(2):262-272. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2020.1747070Links ]

47. Bonavita S, Sacco R, Della Corte M, Esposito S, Sparaco M, d’Ambrosio A, et al. Computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation improves cognitive performances and induces brain functional connectivity changes in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients: an exploratory study. J Neurol . 2015;262(1):91-100. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-014-7528-zLinks ]

48. Bove RM, Rush G, Zhao C, Rowles W, Garcha P, Morrissey J, et al. A Videogame-Based Digital Therapeutic to Improve Processing Speed in People with Multiple Sclerosis: A Feasibility Study. Neurol Ther . 2019;8(1):135-145. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40120-018-0121-0Links ]

49. Chiaravalloti ND, Goverover Y, Costa SL, DeLuca J. A Pilot Study Examining Speed of Processing Training (SPT) to Improve Processing Speed in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis. Frontiers in Neurology . 2018;9- 685. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.00685Links ]

50. Pérez-Martín MY, González-Platas M, Eguía-Del Río P, Croissier-Elías C, Jiménez Sosa A. Efficacy of a short cognitive training program in patients with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat .2017;13:245-52. doi: https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S124448Links ]

51. Pedullà L, Brichetto G, Tacchino A, Vassallo C, Zaratin P, Battaglia MA, et al. Adaptive vs. non-adaptive cognitive training by means of a personalized App: a randomized trial in people with multiple sclerosis. J Neuroeng Rehabil . 2016;13:88. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-016-0193-yLinks ]

52. Blair M, Goveas D, Safi A, Marshall C, Rosehart H, Orenczuk S, et al. Does cognitive training improve attention/working memory in persons with MS? A pilot study using the Cogmed Working Memory Training program. Mult Scler Relat Disord . 2021; 49:102770. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2021.102770Links ]

53. Rahmani M, Rahimian Boogar I, Talepasand S, Nokani M. Comparing the Effectiveness of Computer-Based, Manual-based, and Combined Cognitive Rehabilitation on Cognitive Functions in Relapsing-Remitting Multiple Sclerosis Patients. Basic Clin Neurosci . 2020;11(1):99-110. doi: https://doi.org/10.32598/bcn.9.10.430Links ]

54. Motter JN, Grinberg A, Lieberman DH, Iqnaibi WB, Sneed JR. Computerized cognitive training in young adults with depressive symptoms: Effects on mood, cognition, and everyday functioning. J Affect Disord. . 2019;245:28-37. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.10.109Links ]

55. Ronold EH, Joormann J, Hammar Å. Computerized Working Memory Training in Remission From Major Depressive Disorder: Effects on Emotional Working Memory, Processing Speed, Executive Functions, and Associations With Symptoms. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience . 2022;16:887596. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.887596Links ]

56. Hagen BI, Stubberud J. Goal Management Training and Computerized Cognitive Training in Depression-a 2-Year Follow-Up of a Randomized Controlled Trial. Frontiers in Psychiatry . 2021;12. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.737518Links ]

57. Listunova L, Kienzle J, Bartolovic M, Jaehn A, Grützner TM, Wolf RC, et al. Cognitive remediation therapy for partially remitted unipolar depression: A single-blind randomized controlled trial. J Affect Disord .2020;276:316-26. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.008Links ]

58. Hotton M, Derakshan N, Fox E. A randomised controlled trial investigating the benefits of adaptive working memory training for working memory capacity and attentional control in high worriers. Behav Res Ther . 2018;100:67-77. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.011Links ]

59. Sánchez-López A, De Raedt R, van Put J, Koster EHW. A novel process-based approach to improve resilience: Effects of computerized mouse-based (gaze)contingent attention training (MCAT) on reappraisal and rumination. Behav Res Ther . 2019;118:110-120. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.04.005Links ]

60. Fonzo GA, Fine NB, Wright RN, Achituv M, Zaiko YV, Merin O, et al. Internet-delivered computerized cognitive & affective remediation training for the treatment of acute and chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: Two randomized clinical trials. J Psychiatr Res 2019;115:82-89. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.05.007Links ]

61. McBride RL, Horsfield S, Sandler CX, Cassar J, Casson S, Cvejic E, et al. Cognitive remediation training improves performance in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychiatry Res . 2017;257:400-405. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.035Links ]

62. Clausen AN, Thelen J, Francisco AJ, Bruce J, Martin L, McDowd J, Aupperle RL. Computer-Based Executive Function Training for Combat Veterans With PTSD: A Pilot Clinical Trial Assessing Feasibility and Predictors of Dropout. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:62. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00062Links ]

63. Van de Ven RM, Schmand B, Groet E, Veltman DJ, Murre JMJ. The effect of computer-based cognitive flexibility training on recovery of executive function after stroke: rationale, design and methods of the TAPASS study. BMC Neurol . 2015;15:144. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0397-yLinks ]

64. Van de Ven RM, Murre JMJ, Buitenweg JIV, Veltman DJ, Aaronson JA, Nijboer TCW, et al. The influence of computer-based cognitive flexibility training on subjective cognitive well-being after stroke: A multi-center randomized controlled trial. PLoS One . 2017;12(11):e0187582. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187582Links ]

65. Välimäki M, Mishina K, Kaakinen JK, Holm SK, Vahlo J, Kirjonen M, et al. Digital Gaming for Improving the Functioning of People With Traumatic Brain Injury: Randomized Clinical Feasibility Study. J Med Internet Res . 2018;20(3):e77. doi: https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.7618Links ]

66. De Luca R, Aragona B, Leonardi S, Torrisi M, Galletti B, Galletti F, et al. Computerized Training in Poststroke Aphasia: What About the Long-Term Effects? A Randomized Clinical Trial. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis.2018;27(8):2271-2276. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2018.04.019Links ]

67. Fernandez E, Bergado Rosado JA, Rodriguez Perez D, Salazar Santana S, Torres Aguilar M, Bringas ML. Effectiveness of a Computer-Based Training Program of Attention and Memory in Patients with Acquired Brain Damage. Behav Sci (Basel) . 2017;8(1):4. doi: https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8010004Links ]

68. Oliveira J, Gamito P, Lopes B, Silva AR, Galhordas J, Pereira E, et al. Computerized cognitive training using virtual reality on everyday life activities for patients recovering from stroke. Disabil Rehabil Assist Technol . 2022;17(3):298-303. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/17483107.2020.1749891Links ]

69. Mitrovic A, Mathews M, Ohlsson S, Holland J, McKinlay A. Computer-Based Post-Stroke Rehabilitation of Prospective Memory. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition . 2016;5(2):204-214. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.03.006Links ]

70. De Luca R, Leonardi S, Spadaro L, Russo M, Aragona B, Torrisi M, et al. Improving Cognitive Function in Patients with Stroke: Can Computerized Training Be the Future? J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis . 2018;27(4):1055-1060. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2017.11.008Links ]

71. Dundon NM, Dockree SP, Buckley V, Merriman N, Carton M, Clarke S, et al. Impaired auditory selective attention ameliorated by cognitive training with graded exposure to noise in patients with traumatic brain injury. . Neuropsychologia. 2015;75:74-87. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.012Links ]

72. Khemiri L, Brynte C, Stunkel A, Klingberg T, Jayaram-Lindström N. Working Memory Training in Alcohol Use Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. . 2019;43(1):135-46. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13910Links ]

73. Rass O, Schacht RL, Buckheit K, Johnson MW, Strain EC, Mintzer MZ. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of working memory training in methadone maintenance patients. Drug Alcohol Depend . 2015;156:38-46. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.012Links ]

74. Brooks SJ, Wiemerslage L, Burch KH, Maiorana SA, Cocolas E, Schiöth HB, et al. The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). . 2017;234(12):1911-1921. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4597-6Links ]

75. Brooks SJ, Burch KH, Maiorana SA, Cocolas E, Schioth HB, Nilsson EK, et al. Psychological intervention with working memory training increases basal ganglia volume: A VBM study of inpatient treatment for methamphetamine use. Neuroimage Clin . 2016;12:478-91. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.019Links ]

76. Marceau EM, Berry J, Lunn J, Kelly PJ, Solowij N. Cognitive remediation improves executive functions, self-regulation and quality of life in residents of a substance use disorder therapeutic community. Drug Alcohol Depend . 2017;178:150-8. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.04.023Links ]

77. Valls-Serrano C, Caracuel A, Verdejo-Garcia A. Goal Management Training and Mindfulness Meditation improve executive functions and transfer to ecological tasks of daily life in polysubstance users enrolled in therapeutic community treatment. Drug Alcohol Depend . 2016;165:9-14. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.04.040Links ]

78. Lewis B, Garcia CC, Price JL, Schweizer S, Nixon SJ. Cognitive training in recently-abstinent individuals with alcohol use disorder improves emotional stroop performance: Evidence from a randomized pilot trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2022;231:109239. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.109239 Links ]

79. Loughead J, Falcone M, Wileyto EP, Albelda B, Audrain-McGovern J, Cao W, et al. Can brain games help smokers quit?: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Drug Alcohol Depend . 2016;168:112-8. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.08.621 Links ]

80. Kolodny T, Ashkenazi Y, Farhi M, Shalev L. Computerized Progressive Attention Training (CPAT) vs. Active Control in Adults with ADHD. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement . 2017;1(4):526-538. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0056-x Links ]

81. Liu ZX, Glizer D, Tannock R, Woltering S. EEG alpha power during maintenance of information in working memory in adults with ADHD and its plasticity due to working memory training: A randomized controlled trial. Clin Neurophysiol . 2016;127(2):1307-20. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2015.10.032 Links ]

82. Mawjee K, Woltering S, Tannock R. Working Memory Training in Post-Secondary Students with ADHD: A Randomized Controlled Study. PLoS One . 2015;10(9):e0137173. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137173Links ]

83. Best MW, Gale D, Tran T, Haque MK, Bowie CR. Brief executive function training for individuals with severe mental illness: Effects on EEG synchronization and executive functioning. Schizophr Res . 2019; 203:32-40. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.08.052 Links ]

84. Kashyap H, Reddy P, Mandadi S, Narayanaswamy JC, Sudhir PM, Reddy YCJ. Cognitive training for neurocognitive and functional impairments in obsessive compulsive disorder: A case report. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders . 2019;23:100480. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2019.100480 Links ]

85. Wu LM, Amidi A, Tanenbaum ML, Winkel G, Gordon WA, Hall SJ, et al. Computerized cognitive training in prostate cancer patients on androgen deprivation therapy: A pilot study. Support Care Cancer . 2018;26(6):1917-1926. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-017-4026-8 Links ]

86. Von Ah D, McDonald BC, Crouch AD, Ofner S, Perkins S, Storey S, Considine R, Unverzagt F. Randomized double-masked controlled trial of cognitive training in breast cancer survivors: a preliminary study. Support Care Cancer . 2022:(9):7457-7467. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-022-07182-4 Links ]

87. Bellens A, Roelant E, Sabbe B, Peeters M, van Dam PA. A video-game based cognitive training for breast cancer survivors with cognitive impairment: A prospective randomized pilot trial. Breast . 2020;53:23-32. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.breast.2020.06.003 Links ]

88. Damholdt MF, Mehlsen M, O’Toole MS, Andreasen RK, Pedersen AD, Zachariae R. Web-based cognitive training for breast cancer survivors with cognitive complaints-a randomized controlled trial. Psychooncology. . 2016;25(11):1293-300. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.4058 Links ]

89. Bartlett DM, Govus A, Rankin T, Lampit A, Feindel K, Poudel G, Teo WP, Lo J, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Ziman MR, Cruickshank TM. The effects of multidisciplinary rehabilitation on neuroimaging, biological, cognitive and motor outcomes in individuals with premanifest Huntington's disease. Journal of the neurological sciences . 2020;416:117022. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2020.117022 Links ]

90. Yhnell E, Furby H, Lowe RS, Brookes-Howell LC, Drew CJG, Playle R, et al. A randomised feasibility study of computerised cognitive training as a therapeutic intervention for people with Huntington’s disease (CogTrainHD). Pilot and Feasibility Studies . 2020;6(1):88. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40814-020-00623-z Links ]

91. Towe SL, Patel P, Meade CS. The Acceptability and Potential Utility of Cognitive Training to Improve Working Memory in Persons Living With HIV: A Preliminary Randomized Trial. J Assoc Nurses AIDS Care . 2017;28(4):633-43. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jana.2017.03.007 Links ]

92. Towe SL, Hartsock JT, Xu Y, Meade CS. Web-based cognitive training to improve working memory in persons with co-occurring HIV infection and cocaine use disorder: Outcomes from a randomized controlled trial. AIDS Behav . 2020; 25(5):1542-1551. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-020-02993-0 Links ]

93. Linares R, Borella E, Lechuga MT, Carretti B, Pelegrina S. Nearest transfer effects of working memory training: A comparison of two programs focused on working memory updating. PLOS ONE . 2019;14(2):e0211321. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0211321 Links ]

94. Waris O, Soveri A, Laine M. Transfer after Working Memory Updating Training. PLOS ONE . 2015;10(9): e0138734. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138734 Links ]

95. Schwarb H, Nail J, Schumacher EH. Working memory training improves visual short-term memory capacity. Psychological Research . 2016;80(1):128-48. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0648-y Links ]

96. Fellman D, Jylkkä J, Waris O, Soveri A, Ritakallio L, Haga S, et al. The role of strategy use in working memory training outcomes. Journal of Memory and Language . 2020;110:104064. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104064 Links ]

97. Gordon S, Todder D, Deutsch I, Garbi D, Alkobi O, Shriki O, et al. Effects of neurofeedback and working memory-combined training on executive functions in healthy young adults. Psychol Res . 2020;84(6):1586-609. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01170-w Links ]

98. Xiu L, Zhou R, Jiang Y. Working memory training improves emotion regulation ability: Evidence from HRV. Physiol Behav . 2016;155:25-9. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.004 Links ]

99. Maraver MJ, Bajo MT, Gomez-Ariza CJ. Training on Working Memory and Inhibitory Control in Young Adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience . 2016;10. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00588 Links ]

100. Cohen N, Margulies DS, Ashkenazi S, Schaefer A, Taubert M, Henik A, et al. Using executive control training to suppress amygdala reactivity to aversive information. Neuroimage . 2016;125:1022-1031. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.069 Links ]

101. Namratha HG, George VM, Bajaj G, Mridula J, Bhat JS. Effect of yoga and working memory training on cognitive communicative abilities among middle aged adults. Complement Ther Clin Pract . 2017;28:92-100. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctcp.2017.05.007 Links ]

102. Tallus J, Soveri A, Hämäläinen H, Tuomainen J, Laine M. Effects of Auditory Attention Training with the Dichotic Listening Task: Behavioural and Neurophysiological Evidence. PLOS ONE . 2015;10(10):e0139318. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0139318 Links ]

103. Bennike IH, Wieghorst A, Kirk U. Online-based Mindfulness Training Reduces Behavioral Markers of Mind Wandering. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement . 2017;1(2):172-181. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0020-9 Links ]

104. Olfers KJF, Band GPH. Game-based training of flexibility and attention improves task-switch performance: near and far transfer of cognitive training in an EEG study. Psychol Res . 2018;82(1):186-202. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0933-z Links ]

105. Etherton JL, Oberle CD, Rhoton J, Ney A. Effects of Cogmed working memory training on cognitive performance. Psychol Res . 2019;83(7):1506-18. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1012-9 Links ]

106. Zwilling CE, Daugherty AM, Hillman CH, Kramer AF, Cohen NJ, Barbey AK. Enhanced decision-making through multimodal training. npj Sci Learn . 2019;4(1):1-10. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-019-0049-x Links ]

107. Baniqued PL, Allen CM, Kranz MB, Johnson K, Sipolins A, Dickens C, et al. Working Memory, Reasoning, and Task Switching Training: Transfer Effects, Limitations, and Great Expectations? PLOS ONE . 2015;10(11):e0142169. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142169 Links ]

108. Daugherty AM, Zwilling C, Paul EJ, Sherepa N, Allen C, Kramer AF, et al. Multi-modal fitness and cognitive training to enhance fluid intelligence. Intelligence . 2018;66:32-43. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.11.001 Links ]

109. Wells A, Parong J, Mayer RE. Limits on Training Inhibitory Control with a Focused Video Game. J Cogn Enhanc . 2021;5(1):83-98. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-020-00184-2 Links ]

110. Shahar N, Pereg M, Teodorescu AR, Moran R, Karmon-Presser A, Meiran N. Formation of abstract task representations: Exploring dosage and mechanisms of working memory training effects. Cognition . 2018;181:151-159. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.007 Links ]

111. Schmicker M, Müller P, Schwefel M, Müller NG. Attentional Filter Training but Not Memory Training Improves Decision-Making. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience . 2017;11:138. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00138 Links ]

112. Zhao X, Wang H, Maes JHR. Training and transfer effects of extensive task-switching training in students. Psychological Research . 2020;84(2):389-403. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1059-7 Links ]

113. Hilbert S, Schwaighofer M, Zech A, Sarubin N, Arendasy M, Bühner M. Working memory tasks train working memory but not reasoning: A material- and operation-specific investigation of transfer from working memory practice. Intelligence . 2017;61:102-114. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.01.010 Links ]

114. Pereg M, Shahar N, Meiran N. Can we learn to learn? The influence of procedural working-memory training on rapid instructed-task-learning. Psychological Research. . 2019;83:132-46. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1122-4 Links ]

115. Hogrefe AB, Studer-Luethi B, Kodzhabashev S, Perrig WJ. Mechanisms Underlying N-back Training: Response Consistency During Training Influences Training Outcome. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement . 2017;4(1):406-18. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0042-3 Links ]

116. Foster JL, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Draheim C, Redick TS, Engle RW. Do the effects of working memory training depend on baseline ability level? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. . 2017;43(11):1677-1689. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000426 Links ]

117. Flegal KE, Ragland JD, Ranganath C. Adaptive task difficulty influences neural plasticity and transfer of training. NeuroImage . 2019;188:111-121. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.003 Links ]

118. Talanow T, Ettinger U. Effects of task repetition but no transfer of inhibitory control training in healthy adults. Acta Psychol (Amst) . 2018;187:37-53. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.016 Links ]

119. Martinčević M, Vranić A. Casual Game or Cognitive Gain: Multitask Casual Game as a Training for Young Adults. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement. 2020;434-445. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-020-00173-5 Links ]

120. Strobach T, Huestegge L. Evaluating the Effectiveness of Commercial Brain Game Training with Working-Memory Tasks. J Cogn Enhanc . 2017;1(4):539-58. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0053-0Links ]

Cite like this: Robledo-Castro, Carolina; Lerma Castaño, Piedad Rocio; Pachón Ospina, Luis Gerardo. (2023). Rehabilitation Programs of Executive Functions in Young and Middle Adulthood based on Computational Systems: a Scoping Review. Revista de Investigación e Innovación en Ciencias de la Salud. 5(1), 231-272. https://doi.org/10.46634/riics.134

Editor: Fraidy-Alonso Alzate-Pamplona, MSc., https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6342-3444

Copyright: © 2023. Fundación Universitaria María Cano. The Revista de Investigación e Innovación en Ciencias de la Salud provides open access to all its content under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).

Declaration of interests: The authors have declared that there is no conflict of interest.

Data availability: All relevant data is found in the article. For more information, contact the corresponding author.

Financing: None. This research did not receive specific resources from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors.

Disclaimer: The content of this article is the sole responsibility of the authors and does not represent an official opinion of their institutions or of the Revista de Investigación e Innovación en Ciencias de la Salud.

Authors contribution:

Carolina Robledo-Castro: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, supervision, validation, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing.

Piedad Rocio Lerma Castaño: data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, validation, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing.

Luis Gerardo Pachón Ospina: data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, validation, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing.

Appendix A: Descriptive matrix: study characteristics 

Authors Year Country Study design Primary measures (Type of measure implemented) Population profile Participant characteristics Intervention characteristics
No. Middle age Duration in weeks, sessions per week, follow-up Number of sessions; session duration computer based rehabilitation program Program Features
Bell et al. [30] 2018 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups; CG active. WM (NPT, EA) Schizophrenia 77 51.18 26 weeks, NS; 6 and 12 months. 2:00 p.m. to 6:00 p.m.; NS Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Web platform, commercial, gamified
Biagianti et al. [31] 2017 USA Non-randomized, double-blind controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; without CG Global cognition, verbal memory and problem solving (NPT) Schizophrenia 74 49.24 8 weeks, 5; none. 40; 60 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Harvey et al. [32] 2020 USA Crossover diagnostic study; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups; Without CG. PS, WM and attention (NPT) Schizophrenia, depression and bipolarity 49 28.6 NS NS; 45 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Piskulic et al. [33] 2015 Canada Pilot study; between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. PS, attention, vigilance, verbal learning, reasoning and problem solving (NPT) Youth at high risk of psychosis 29 19.72 36 weeks, NS ; none. NS Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Michalopoulou et al. [34] 2015 United Kingdom Randomized, double-blind, controlled clinical trial with placebo; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Executive Functions (NPT) Schizophrenia 48 35.4 2 weeks; NS; none. 19; NS Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Tarasenko et al. [35] 2016 USA Single group design. Auditory PS (NPT) Schizophrenia 37 44.7 1 day; 1; none. 1; 60 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Mahncke et al. [36] 2019 USA Multicenter, double-blind, randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, prospective; Active QA Auditory and visual processing speed, executive function, general cognitive functioning (NPT, NST) Schizophrenia 82 42.9 26 weeks, 5; none. 130; 60 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Jahshan et al. [37] 2019 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups; active CG Executive functioning, WM, Attention, problem solving (NPT, NST, EGG) Schizophrenia 99 51.59 12 weeks, NS; none. 36; NS Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Reder et al. [38] 2017 United Kingdom Single-blind, multicenter, randomized clinical superiority trial; active CG. Memory and executive function (NPT) Schizophrenia 93 38.3 12 weeks, 3; 14 weeks. 40; 60 minutes CIRCUITS Cognitive remediation, gamified, based on activities of daily living
Palumbo et al. [39] 2019 Italy Pilot study; single group design. CF, learning, PS, WM and executive control (NPT) Schizophrenia 17 36,37 12 weels, 3; none. 40; 60 minutes CIRCUITS Cognitive remediation, gamified, based on activities of daily living
Fernández-Gonzalo et al. [40] 2015 Spain Randomized controlled pilot study; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; active CG CF, IC, VF, PS, planning, problem solving, logical memory and visual reproduction (NPT) Schizophrenia 53 30.9 5 weeks; two; 4 weeks. 30; 60 minutes GNPT - NeuroPersonalTrainer Web platform, cognitive remediation, commercial, clinical
Matsuoka et al. [41] 2019 Japan Randomized, blinded controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. VF, verbal memory, TM, motor speed, attention and executive function (NPT, RM) Schizophrenia 31 36.1 12 weeks, 2; none. 24; 60 minutes Jcores Rehabilitation software, cognitive remediation, clinical
Messinis et al. [42] 2017 Greece Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. Episodic memory, PS, attention and executive functions (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 58 46.03 10 weeks, NE ; none. NE Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Messinis et al. [43] 2020 Greece Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. PS, WM, episodic memory (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 36 46.47 8 weeks, 3; none. 24; 45 minutes Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Arian Darestani et al. [44] 2020 Iran Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. VF, verbal learning and memory (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 60 39.23 5 weeks; 2;5 weeks. 10; 60 minutes Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Campbell et al. [45] 2016 United Kingdom Open-label, randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Processing speed, visuospatial memory, and divided attention (NPT, RM) Multiple sclerosis 38 46.21 6 weeks; 3; 12 weeks. 18; 45 minutes Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Naeeni et al. [46] 2022 Iran Pilot study. Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups. Visual attention, working memory, verbal and non-verbal executive functions, processing speed (NPT) multiple sclerosis 54 39.3 5 weeks, 5, none 25, 60 minutes Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Bonavita et al. [47] 2015 Italy Exploratory, open study; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. PS, visual and verbal sustained memory (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 36 48 8 weeks, 2; none. NS; 50 minutes rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Bove e t al. [48] 2019 USA Viability study; single group design Executive functions and PS (NPT, NST) Multiple sclerosis 21 53,8 4 weeks; 5;none. 20; 25 minutes Neurorocer - Proyecto EVO Commercial rehabilitation platform, gamified, applied at home
Chiaravalloti et al. [49] 2022 United States This randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. between subjects. (pre-post), 2 groups, CG with placebo. Processing speed (NPT) multiple sclerosis 84 52.3 5 weeks, 2, 6 months 60 minutes Speed Processing Training (SOPT) Computerized novel.
Perez-Martin et al. [50] 2017 Spain Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. Attention, PS, memory, executive functions (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 62 44.93 NS 12; 60 to 75 minutes NS Novel, designed by researchers
Pedulla et al. [51] 2016 Italy Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. WM (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 28 47.5 8 weeks, 5; none. 40; 30 minutes COGNI-TRAcK - Cognitive Training Kit Rehabilitation, institutional, clinical software
Blair et al. [52] 2021 Canada Pilot study. Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, TAU CG. Inhibition, attention. (NPT) multiple sclerosis 30 51.1 5 weeks, 5, none 25, 60 minutes CWWM - Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Rahmani et al. [53] 2020 Iran Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 5 groups, factorial; Active and passive CG. WM, selective attention, executive function, and PS (NPT) Multiple sclerosis 60 29.65 20 weeks, 1; 2 months. twenty-one; 60 minutes Captain's Log, Mindpower Commercial, gamified, computerized cognitive rehabilitation training (CCRT)
Motter et al. [54] 2019 USA Randomized, controlled, double-blind clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups; Active CG. PS and daily operation (NPT) depressive disorders 46 twenty-one 8 weeks, 5; none. 40; 15 minutes Peak commercial, gamified
Ronold et al. [55] 2022 Norway Pilot study. Between subjects (pre-post); 2 only group Executive functions, emotional working memory, inhibition, processing speed (NPT) Depression 20 36.2 5 weeks, 5; none. 25, 45 minutes CWWM - Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Hagen et al., [56] 2021 does not record Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups. Active CG (Goal management training) Depression. (NPT) self-perceived executive functions 43 43.2 4 weeks, 2, 6 months and 2 years 9, NS. NS NS
Listunova et al. [57] 2020 Germany Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, parallel; Passive CG. Divided and selective attention, alertness, TM, response inhibition, and planning (NP) depressive disorders 57 45.39 5 weeks; 3; none. 8 to 15, 60 minutes Cogniplus Intelligent, commercial, gamified interactive system
Hotton et al. [58] 2018 UK Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. TM and control of attention (NP) Anxiety disorder 41 29.2 2 weeks; NE; none. fifteen; NS n-back training Novel, based on classical tasks, n-back task
Sanchez et al. [59] 2019 Spain proof of concept; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; active CG. Emotional regulation (NPT, NST, EA) Emotional disorders related to stress. 41 22.1 1 day; 1; none. 1; NS ECAT - advanced gaze contingent attention training Novel, active contingent attention training (MCAT) training
Fonzo et al. [60] 2019 Israel Randomized, blinded controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 4 groups, parallel; CG active. Sustained attention, task switching, inhibition, PS, verbal memory, emotional regulation. (NPT, PM) post traumatic stress disorder 164 36.08 5 weeks; NE; none. 30; 30 to 45 minutes NS Novel, based on video games
McBride et al. [61] 2017 Australia Preliminary Study, Single Group Design Attention, WM, processing speed, and executive functioning (NPT) Chronic Fatigue Syndrome 36 36 12 weeks, 11; none. NS Lumosity commercial, gamified
Clausen et al. [62] 2019 USA Pilot study; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, nonrandomized, parallel; CG active. Planning, inhibition, memory, PS, CF, verbal attention. (NPT,RM) post traumatic stress disorder 40 32.6 6 weeks; 5;none. 30; 30 minutes Lumosity Commercial, gamified, applied at home
Van deVen et al. [63] 2015 Netherlands Randomized, multicenter, double-blind controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Active and passive CG. CF and executive function (NPT) stroke 120 30 to 80 years 12 weeks, 5; none. 58; 30 to 45 minutes BrainGymmer Online trading platform, gamified
Van deVen et al. [64] 2017 Netherlands Randomized, controlled, double-blind clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Passive CG. Executive functioning, CF, WM attention, reasoning (NPT) stroke 87 57 12 weeks, NE; 4 weeks. 58; 30 minutes BrainGymmer trading platform , gamified
Valimaki et al. [65] 2018 Finlandia Feasibility study, double blind; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Active and passive CG. PS, attention, executive functions, and TM (NPT) acquired brain damage 90 42.14 8 weeks, 7; none. NS; 30 minutes Cognifit Multi-device platform, Commercial, gamified
DeLuca et al. [66] 2018 Italy Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups; active CG VF, Reversive Motor Learning, instrumental activities of daily living (NP) Ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke, presence of cognitive impairment 35 42.15 8 weeks, 6; none. 24; 45 minutes Erica Web platform, cognitive remediation, commercial, clinical
Fernandez et al. [67] 2018 Cuba Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Attention, memory, visuospatial processing, and executive functions (NPT) acquired brain damage 80 33.7 8 weeks, 5; none. NS; 50 minutes Rehacom Commercial, Clinical, modular therapy system
Oliveira et al. [68] 2022 Portugal Pilot study. Between subjects (pre-post); only group. Acquired Brain Injury (CVA) (NPT) Flexibility, inhibition, working memory, attention 30 60 5 weeks 10, NS Systemic lisbon battery Virtual reality based on tasks of daily life.
Mitrovic et al. [69] 2016 New Zealand Single group design. Prospective memory (NPT) Stroke 15 65 4 weeks; two; none. 10; 60 minutes Visual imagery training Novel, designed by the authors, based on visual image training
DeLuca et al. [70] 2018 Italy Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Selective attention, TM, abstraction capacity, flexibility (NPT) Post-stroke aphasia 32 51.9 8 weeks, 3; 3 months. 24; 45 minutes Power Afa Aphasia Software and Rehabilitation, Commercial, Clinical
Dundon et al. [71] 2015 Ireland Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. Cognitive control, executive control, memory, processing speed and Attention. (NPT, EGG) Traumatic brain injury, impairment in attention and executive control 40 37.3 NS NS APT - Cognitive Training Based on Attention Process Training Based on care training, Institutional, clinical.
Khemiri et al. [72] 2019 Sweden Randomized, controlled, double-blind clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups; CG active. Verbal and visuospatial TM (NP) substance use disorder 50 49.6 5 weeks; 5; none. 25; 30 to 45 minutes CWWM - Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train TM, gamified, applied at home
Rass et al. [73] 2015 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. WM, memory, metamemory, PS, Psychomotor Attention (NPT, NST) substance use disorder 56 43.5 6 weeks; 4;6 months. 25; 45 minutes CWWM - Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Brooks et al. [74] 2017 Sweden Exploratory pilot study; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; CG active. WM (NPT) substance use disorder 66 29.1 4 weeks; NS; none. NS; 30 minutes CYa - Curb Your Addiction Rehabilitation software, clinical, based on N-back paradigm
Brooks et al. [75] 2016 South Africa Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; Passive CG. WM (NPT, RM) substance use disorder 57 28.42 4 weeks; 5;none. twenty; 30 minutes CYa - Curb Your Addiction Rehabilitation software, clinical, based on N-back paradigm
Marceau et al. [76] 2017 Australia Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, sequential, not randomized; Active QA Attention, memory, self-awareness, inhibition, visual and verbal TM, emotion regulation, decision making, and problem solving. (NPT) substance use disorder 33 32.5 4 weeks; 3; none. 12; 120 minutes Lumosity commercial, gamified
Valls-Serrano et al. [77] 2016 Spain Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. WM, CI, decision making, planning (NPT) substance use disorder 32 35.19 8 weeks; two; none 16; 120 minutes Goal Management Training Novel based on goal management
Lewis et al. [78] 2022 United States Pilot study. Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, CG TAU Inhibition, working memory (NPT) Disorders associated with substance use 77 42.6 NS 12, 45 minutes NS Novel based on n-back
Loughead et al. [79] 2016 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Memory, attention, executive function, emotional regulation, inhibition (NPT) smokers 213 43.28 12 weeks, NS; 6 months. NS; 20 minutes NS Cognitive remediation, Novel, based on previous studies
Kolodny et al. [80] 2017 Israel Between subjects (pre-post), blind; 2 randomized groups; CG active. Sustained, selective attention, conflict resolution (NP) ADHD 30 25.2 8 weeks, 2; none. 16; 60 minutes CPAT - Computerized Progressive Attention Training Program Based on progressive attention training, novel
Liu et al. [81] 2016 Canada Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Active and passive CG. TM (NPT, EGG) ADHD 99 24.21 5 weeks; 5; none. 25; 45 minutes Cogmed Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Mawjee et al. [82] 2015 Canada Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Active and passive CG. WM (NPT, NST) Post-secondary students with ADHD 97 23.9 5 weeks; 5; none. 25; 45 minutes CWWM - Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Best et al. [83] 2019 Canada Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; Active QA Executive functions (NPT, EGG) cognitive decline 25 33.18 2 weeks, 3; none. NS; 40 minutes Happy Neuron Pro Web platform, cognitive remediation, commercial, clinical
Kashyap et al. [84] 2019 India Case study Executive functions, verbal fluency, planning, attention, and TM (NPT) Obsessive compulsive disorder 1 29 12 weeks, 1; none. 12; 180 minutes Cogtrain Compensatory cognitive training, open access, gamified
Wu et al. [85] 2018 USA Randomized controlled pilot study; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. Verbal and visual memory (NPT, NST, EA) Cancer 44 66.5 8 weeks, 5; none. NS; 60 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Von et al., [86] 2022 USA Randomized, double-blind controlled trial. Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups. CG active. Self-perceived executive function, working memory, processing speed, verbal fluency (NP) cancer survivors 30 57.6 10 weeks, 4, none 40, 60 minutes Brain HQ, Brain Fitness Online Commercial Platform, gamified
Bellens et al. [87] 2020 Belgium Randomized controlled pilot study; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. Executive function, attention, memory, language and perception (NPT) breast cancer survivors 46 52.1 12 weeks, 3; none. NS; 60 minutes Aquasnap - Mycognition commercial, gamified
Damholdt et al. [88] 2016 Denmark Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. Attention, PS, learning, memory, problem solving (NPT, EA) History of breast cancer 157 54.98 6 weeks; 5; 5 months. 30; 30 minutes HappyNeuronPro Web platform, commercial, clinical
Bartlet et al. [89] 2020 Australia Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active; Passive CG. Verbal learning, memory, attention, cognitive flexibility, and processing speed (NPT, RS, PM) Huntington's disease 31 NE 6 weeks; NE; 36 months. NS Neuronation commercial, gamified
Yhnell et al. [90] 2020 United Kingdom Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; Passive CG. Executive functions: planning, and cognitive flexibility (NPT, NST, EA) Huntington's disease 26 45.8 12 weeks, 3; none. 36; 30 minutes HappyNeuronPro Web platform, commercial, clinical, applied at home
Towe et al. [91] 2017 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial, Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups; Active QA WM (NPT) HIV, and neurocognitive impairment 21 47.9 10 weeks, 2; none. 12; NE PSSCogRehab Rehabilitation Software, Commercial
Towe et al. [92] 2020 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. WM (NPT) HIV 56 48.62 10 weeks, NE ; none. 48; NE lumosity commercial, gamified
Linares et al. [93] 2019 Spain Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 6 groups, parallel; CG active. TM (NPT) update healthy 193 21.36 NS 6; 30 minutes n-back training Novel, based on classical tasks, n-back task
Waris et al. [94] 2015 Finlandia Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; CG active. WM (NPT) healthy 31 24.16 5 weeks; 3; none. NS; 45 minutes NS Novel, based on classic tasks, n-back tasks, Jaeggi tasks
Schwarb et al. [95] 2016 USA single group design WM, short term memory, Attention control (NPT, NST) healthy 53 20.39 4 weeks; two; none. 8; 60 minutes n-back training Novel, based on classical tasks, n-back task
Felman et al. [96] 2020 Finlandia Large scale randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel. WM (SNP) healthy 258 32.74 4 weeks; NS; none. 12; 30 minutes n-back training Novel, based on classical tasks, n-back task
Gordon et al. [97] 2019 Israel Between subjects (pre-post); 6 groups, factorial; Passive CG. CI, WM, CF (NPT, EGG) Israel Defense Force soldiers healthy 140 22.12 5 weeks; two; 4 weeks. 10; 60 minutes NS Novel, based on WWM task developed by Shahar and Meiran, N-back paradigm
Xiu et al. [98] 2016 China Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; Passive CG. TM (NST, EGG) healthy college students 40 21.7 4 weeks; 5;none. 20 to 30 minutes Running Working Memory Novel, based on classic tasks
Maraver et al. [99] 2016 Spain Between subjects (pre-post); 4 groups, parallel; CG active. WM, CI, executive control (NPT) healthy 112 20.51 2 weeks, 2; none. 6; 120 minutes NS Novel, based on classical tasks, stroop task
Cohen et al. [100] 2016 Germany Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized. Attention, emotional control, IQ (RM) healthy 26 25 1 week; 18, several sessions a day; none. NS; 15 minutes NS Novel, based on classic tasks, flanker task
Namratha et al. [101] 2017 India Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, factorial, randomized; Passive CG. WM (NPT, EA) healthy 45 51 NE 10; 45 minutes NS Novel, based on classical tasks, TM training
Tallus et al. [102] 2015 Finlandia Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; CG active. Hearing care and TM (NPT, PE) healthy 26 20 to 34 years 4 weeks; 4; none. 16; 25 minutes NS Novel, based on classical tasks, dichotic listening task
Bennike et al. [103] 2017 Denmark Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Attention, concentration, mental distraction, dispositional mindfulness (NP) healthy 95 41.4 4 weeks; 7;none. 30, 20 minutes Lumosity commercial, gamified
Olfers et al. [104] 2017 Netherlands Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; CG active. CF, Attention and TM (NPT, EGG) healthy 72 23 6 weeks; NE, none. twenty; 45 minutes Lumosity commercial, gamified
Etherton et al. [105] 2019 USA Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; Passive CG. WM (NPT, NST) healthy college students 92 20.07 5 weeks; 5; none. 25; 40 minutes Cogmed Working Memory Training Commercial platform to train WM, gamified
Zwilling et al. [106] 2019 USA Between subjects (pre-post); 4 groups, parallel; CG active. Executive functions, visuospatial TM, and analogical reasoning. (NPT) healthy 160 23.8 16 weeks, 3; none. 48; 70 minutes Mind Frontiers commercial, gamified
Baniqued et al. [107] 2015 USA Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; CG active. WM (NPT, NST, RM) healthy 90 20.8 5 weeks; 4; none. twenty; 75 minutes Mind Frontiers commercial, gamified
Daugherty et al. [108] 2019 USA Large-scale cluster randomized controlled clinical trial. Between subjects (pre-post); 4 groups; active CG Visuospatial TM, executive functions, and analogical reasoning (NPT, PM) healthy 2. 3. 4 23.35 16 weeks, 3; none. 40; 75 minutes Mind Frontiers commercial, gamified
Wells et al. [109] 2020 USA Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, not randomized; CG active. IC (NPT) healthy 52 18.98 2 weeks, 2; none. 4; 30 minutes Gwakkamole Video game, commercial
Shahar et al. [110] 2018 Israel Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, randomized, parallel; Active and passive CG. Procedural TM, Declarative TM, Reasoning (NPT) healthy college students 175 23.32 7 weeks, 2; none. 14; 30 minutes NS Novel, procedural TM training
Schmicker et al. [111] 2017 Germany. Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; CG active. WM, attention filtering, decision making, inhibition (PEN) healthy 29 24.31 1 week; 5; none. 5; NE NS Novel, designed by the authors
Zhao et al. [112] 2020 China Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, not randomized; CG active. Task Change (NPT) healthy college students 60 19.85 NE 12; NE NS Novel, based on classic tasks
Hilbert et al. [113] 2017 Germany Between subjects (pre-post), double blind; 7 groups, parallel; CG active and passive. WM, reasoning (NP) healthy college students 216 22 2 weeks, 6; none. 12, 20 minutes NS Novel, based on Oberauer's tasks
Pereg et al. [114] 2019 Israel Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; Active and passive CG. Procedural TM (NST) University students 175 23.32 10 weeks, NE ; none. 12 to 14; 30 minutes NS Novel, procedural TM training
Hogrefe et al. [115] 2017 Swiss Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; Passive CG. WM (NPT, NST) healthy college students 137 22.93 4 weeks; 5;none. twenty; 15 minutes BrainTwister2 Novel, platform independent application, based on N-back tasks
Foster et al. [116] 2017 Australia Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel, randomized; CG active. WM (NPT, NST) healthy college students 143 24.56 NS 20 to 75 minutes NS Novel, verbal and spatial TM tasks
Flegal to. [117] 2019 USA Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post); 2 groups, parallel; Active and passive CG. WM (NPT, RM) healthy 63 20.8 3 weeks; 4; none. 12; NS NS Novel, based on classic tasks, TM refresher training
Talanow et al. [118] 2018 Germany Randomized controlled clinical trial; Between subjects (pre-post); 3 groups, parallel; CG active and passive. Inhibition, executive functions (NPT) healthy 102 22.49 NS 8; NS NS Novel, based on classical tasks, stroop task
Martincevic et al. [119] 2020 Croatia Randomized controlled clinical trial, single blind; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups; CG active. Displacement, update, inhibition, executive functions (NPT) healthy 73 20.46 4 weeks; 3;6 months. 12, 15 to 20 minutes Multitask game serious games, commercial
Strobach et al. [120] 2017 Germany Large scale controlled study; Between subjects (pre-post), 2 groups, randomized; CG active. Memory, inhibition (NPT) healthy 176 42.8 NS twenty-one; 35 minutes Neuronation Commercial, gamified, applied at home

Note: N: Sample size; NPT: neuropsychological tests; EGG: Electroencephalogram; MRI: Magnetic resonance imaging; EA: Interviews and self-reports; NST: non-standardized tests; PM: Physiological measures; SF: Self reports; EP: evoked potentials; NS: Not specified; ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. CG: control group; TAU: treatment as usua; WM: Working memory; PS: Processing speed; VF: Verbal Fluency; IC: inhibitory control; CF: cognitive flexibility.

Received: April 12, 2022; Revised: July 17, 2022; Accepted: January 19, 2023

*Correspondence: Carolina Robledo-Castro. Email: crobledoc@ut.edu.co

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License